Measures of visual acuity (VA) and assessment for higher visual function deficits (HVFDs) are critical components for the diagnosis and management of cerebral visual impairment (CVI). VA, a common measure of a “visual function” (i.e., of how the eye functions), forms the basis for defining visual loss and disability from ocular disease and, by extension, visual impairment from brain-based visual loss, which affects how the individual functions in their environment or “functional vision” (Colenbrander, 2003; Bennett et al., 2019). CVI, a brain-based condition, leads to challenges with functional vision. VA measures are limited to the narrow assessment of one aspect of visual function (i.e., spatial resolution) in conditions of high luminance and contrast but inadequate for a comprehensive assessment of other visual functions (e.g., contrast sensitivity). Importantly VA measures do not assess how vision is utilized in everyday visual activities mediated through higher visual functions (HVFs). While terminology in the field is variable, for example, higher visual functions (Rolls, 1991) and higher-level visual perception (Itzhak et al., 2023), we have used HVFDs (Zihl and Kennard, 2003) to characterize higher visual processing deficits ascribed to the dorsal and ventral streams. Deficits in processing within the inner retina and optic nerve are typically classified as ocular problems, whereas brain-related visual processing deficits fall under the category of HVFDs (Colenbrander, 2010a; Ortibus et al., 2011, 2019; Bennett et al., 2019). For determining HVFDs, an assessment of HVFs or visually guided behaviors is necessary (Dutton and Jacobson, 2001; Bennett et al., 2019; Ortibus et al., 2019). VA and HVFs are both affected in CVI and significant HVFDs may be present in presence of normal VA and vice versa (Chandna et al., 2021; van Genderen et al., 2012; Bosch et al., 2014). Historically, CVI was diagnosed on the basis of VA alone (Marquis, 1934; Whiting et al., 1985). However, a review of literature regarding the terminology and manifestations of CVI (Hoyt, 2003; Colenbrander, 2010a; Jacobsen, 2014; McConnell et al., 2021) and the development of a consensus definition of CVI (Sakki et al., 2017) strongly suggests the inclusion of assessment of HVFDs in the management and the need for this change to be explicitly reflected in the international classification systems of visual impairment (Colenbrander, 2010b; Ravenscroft, 2016; Sakki et al., 2017; Kran et al., 2019).
HVFs can be explained through a functional model of two distinct but interconnected cerebral networks; the dorsal stream connecting occipital and largely posterior parietal lobes (occipital-parietal pathway) (Gallivan and Goodale, 2018; Weiller et al., 2021) and the ventral stream connecting the occipital and inferotemporal cortical area (occipital-temporal pathway) (Weiller et al., 2021). The dorsal stream executes visually guided action, which is termed the pathway for action. The ventral stream aids in the recognition of shapes, objects, and faces, which is termed the pathway for cognition (Milner, 2017; Goodale et al., 2005). There are anatomical interconnections between the dorsal and ventral stream such as the vertical occipital fasciculus (Yeatman et al., 2014; Bauer and Merabet, 2024), which serve to integrate higher visual functions such as reach and grasp action. Additional white matter pathways to cortical areas such as frontal lobe (Ortibus et al., 2012; Bauer and Merabet, 2024; Sarubbo et al., 2013; Dutton et al., 2004) and sub-cortical areas (Merabet et al., 2017), especially superior colliculus (Sprague and Meikle, 1965), contribute to multisensory integration (Chokron and Dutton, 2016), visuomotor execution (Polanen and Davare, 2015), and eye movement control (Chang and Borchert, 2021) to complete visual perception and action.
From this model, it is possible to build a hypothesis that the loss of VA and HVFs may be affected selectively or together but not be interdependent. Though van Genderen et al. (2012) and Chandna et al. (2021) reported HVFDs in the presence of near-normal VA in children with CVI, they did not study the relationship between HVFDs and different levels of VA loss in CVI. VA measures and HVFDs have been reported as part of a study, but the relationship has not been directly examined (Morelli et al., 2022) and the specific VA results were not correlated with the results of the Children’s Visual Impairment Test for 3- to 6-year-olds (CVIT 3–6) to assess visual perceptual functions (Vancleef et al., 2020). This relationship is important for a holistic view of the visual challenges faced by children with CVI (Philip and Dutton, 2014; Bennett et al., 2019; Kran et al., 2019). For the assessment and intervention, it is important to know if HVFDs are independent of VA. Are HVFDs less affected in children with mild to moderate VA loss? Is the spectrum and severity of HVFDs related to VA measures? Does age matter? The answers to these questions would provide an understanding of the visual capabilities of the child and (re)habilitation measures needed to support the child, their family, the teachers, and the school. It may provide insight into the division and the interrelationship between conscious vision (visual cognition; VA; the ventral stream) and non-conscious vision (dorsal stream, action guided by vision without conscious visual perception or awareness) (Philip and Dutton, 2014) and the possible reasons for the relationship between VA measures and HVFDs.
Previously, children with CVI and normal VA were studied (Chandna et al., 2021) where we reported that normal VA did not preclude HVFDs. In this study, we examine the relationship between HVFDs and different levels of VA loss in CVI to extend the relationship between VA land HVFDs, discuss the implications of the results for CVI, and increase our understanding of visual perception, cognition, and visually guided action.
2 Materials and methodsThis study received the Institutional Review Board approval (CHA002) and abided by the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki. Informed written consent was obtained from parents and assent from children where appropriate.
2.1 Higher visual function question inventory (HVFQI)The HVFQI, a clinical assessment instrument, elicits answers to questions on visual behaviors associated with higher visual function deficits (visual perceptual deficits) in CVI. The original clustering of questions into putative domains was retained from the original CVI-I (Dutton and Bax, 2010; Macintyre-Béon et al., 2010, 2012; Ortibus et al., 2011; Philip et al., 2016). The HVFQI had been modified as described in Chandna et al. (2021) from CVI-I (Dutton et al., 2010; Macintyre-Béon et al., 2012). An additional seven questions were added to this HVFQI following a recent review of literature and discussion with Professor Gordon Dutton. However, for an accurate comparison and analysis, we report on the same 51 questions that were included in Chandna et al. (2021). The responses were tabulated along a Likert scale. Participants chose from Never, Rarely, Sometimes, Often, and Always. A Not Applicable option was included since several of the observed behavior items may not be developmentally appropriate or not assessed due to comorbid conditions. Not Applicable responses were accounted for in the analysis as described in Chandna et al. (2021). The HVFQI was delivered through remote video interviews with a web-based application—the HVFQI web app.
2.2 RecruitmentStudy participants were recruited through clinical institutions, educational networks, CVI support groups, information websites and social media groups, parents, teachers of the visually impaired, colleagues, and friends. Following an expression of interest in the research project by the parent or caregiver, the research information and consent and assent documents were provided for their review. A face-to-face virtual meeting (Zoom) was set up across time zones at a mutually convenient time, At the meeting, informed digital e-consent and consent for sharing of clinical information was obtained. Assent, with parental involvement, was obtained for children over 12 years of age, if present at the interview and cognitively appropriate for the child.
2.3 HVFQI interviewThe HVFQI interview via Zoom was commenced with standardized instructions. A unique participant ID was generated. Single, randomly generated questions were displayed on a shared screen to the participant along with the range of possible responses (the Likert scale and the Not Applicable options). The parent was encouraged to read the question and choose one of the options. If requested, questions were clarified with non-leading explanations. Comments from the parent were noted in the free text section on the screen. If the Not Applicable option was chosen, the reason was documented. The HVFQI was completed in one session and within-session breaks were given if necessary. We did not control the response time. While the neurotypical and CVI cohorts were not timed for (Chandna et al., 2021), responses collected using the HVFQI web app were annotated with timestamps. The range of time taken for all respondents via the app (CVI and NT) varied between 6 and 67 min (mean ± SD: 31.15 ± 13.45 min, n = 61). The average questionnaire response time for CVI participants took twice as long (33.60 ± 13.17 min, nCVI = 52) than for neurotypical (NT) participants (15.10 ± 13.32 min, nNT = 9).
2.4 ParticipantsThis study introduced new participants, both neurotypical and those with a CVI diagnosis. The overall analysis included previously reported participants from Chandna et al. (2021) (see Table 1).
Table 1. Combined participant statistics from current study and 2021 study.
2.4.1 Children with CVI groupA total of 59 children (35 boys, 24 girls) with an established diagnosis of cerebral visual impairment (CVI) participated in the study. The history and clinical details of the participating children were obtained through a direct semi-structured interview and documented on our standard form. We recorded a detailed history and present status of vision, details of onset and course of CVI, comorbid conditions, course of pregnancy, birth history, adverse events, and developmental and family history. Clinical documents and reports of investigations including brain imaging were obtained. The diagnosis of CVI, established prior to participation in this study, was confirmed as part of the inclusion process.
The mean age of the participants was 9.95 ± 3.95 years. The mean binocular VA was 0.26 ± 0.29 Log MAR. Eight children (13%) had acuity worse than 1.0 Log MAR. Out of the participating children, 36 (60%) were born at term; three born post-term; and 21 (40%) were born preterm (pregnancy term: one, early; 12, moderate; 4, very preterm; and 4, extremely preterm; World Health Organization classification). The causes of CVI were related to brain injury ranging from cerebral hemorrhage (20; 33%), meningitis (8; 13%), cerebral hypoxia (6; 10%), brain malformation (4; 7%), metabolic disturbance (3; 5%), head trauma (1; 2%), brain tumor (1; 2%,) and genetic causes (17; 28%). There were multiple comorbid conditions such as autism (25; 42%); attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (10, 17%), and central auditory processing deficit (CAPD) (5, 8%). Some children had more than one cause and comorbid condition.
2.4.2 Additional CVI cohort from 2021 studyFor comparison, we included the previous cohort of 33 children with CVI (mean age ± SD: 7.0 ± 2.8 years) and with those near normal VA (0.14 ± 0.12 logMAR) (Chandna et al., 2021). Exploratory data analysis did not reveal any significant differences (except VA) between the 2021 and the present cohort. Therefore, the two cohorts were merged to form the definitive cohort (Table 1). The results in the main body of this article are presented as a combined group.
2.4.3 Neurotypical group includes cohort from 2021 studyThe majority of the neurotypical group (NT) of 111 typically developing children (mean age 8.7 ± 2.8 years and VA of 0.14 ± 0.16 logMAR) were interviewed in (Chandna et al., 2021). An additional nine neurotypical children (3 boys, 6 girls; mean age 9.7 ± 3.4 years) with normal VA responded to the question inventory for this study, extending the reference sample size. All children were assessed with the same detailed medical and vision history (including a detailed interview regarding present and past eye and general health; pregnancy and birth history; past medical and surgical illnesses; family history of eye and general health, especially inherited conditions; a social history; current medication list and results of all available medical tests and investigations) and HVFQI (Chandna et al., 2021; the additional seven questions in the recent HVFQI were not included in the analysis). An initial exploratory comparative analysis of the two groups did not reveal any significant differences. The two groups were combined and the results are presented as a combined group (Table 1).
2.4.4 Combined groupsThe combined cohort (2021 and the present cohort) comprised 92 CVI participants and 120 NTs (see Table 2).
Table 2. Participants’ CVI causes and comorbidities.
2.5 Data collection and storageAll clinical data was transcribed to the database. Non-identifiable HVFQI data collected with the help of the HVFQI web app from each interview was downloaded to a dedicated database. All data was stored on a secure site. Access to the data is granted through authentication credentials and a simple role-based access control security model; all communication to the server was encrypted.
3 Data analysis: objectives, methodology, and resultsIn this section, we present a three-part data analysis. First, we (re)validate the HVFQI with the new cohort of children with CVI with several levels of VA loss building on our previous study where we validated the HVFQI for children with normal VA in comparison with NTs (Section 3.1). Second, we examine the relationship between HVFD and VA (Section 3.2). Finally, we present our early analysis of HVFDs with age (Section 3.3). For the HVFQI analysis, we analyzed the same 51-question HVFQI (Chandna et al., 2021); the additional nine questions in the recent HVFQI were not included in the analysis.
3.1 Objective 1: evaluate the efficacy of the HVFQI questionnaire in identifying and characterizing higher visual function deficits in children with CVI and its ability to distinguish neurotypical childrenTools that are effective at characterizing the HVFDs and that are inexpensive and easily available anywhere in world are essential to achieve impactful and sustained advancements in CVI research and practice. The study by Chandna et al. (2021) introduced one such tool, the HVFQI questionnaire, which was shown to be effective at determining HVFDs. Importantly, Chandna et al. (2021) also demonstrated that the information gathered by the HVFQI questionnaire was effective at identifying children with CVI from neurotypicals. In the current study, we seek to conclusively establish the effectiveness of the HVFQI questionnaire using a substantially larger participant pool. We also introduce two new metrics that quantify the spectrum and severity of HVFDs to provide a two-dimensional view of this complex visual impairment.
3.1.1 Severity of HVFDs 3.1.1.1 MethodologyFor this analysis, we replicate the experimental methodology employed by Chandna et al. (2021). Specifically, the two participant groups being studied (CVI and NT) are compared using the HVFD Severity Index. The HVFD Severity Index is computed for each participant and is defined as the arithmetic mean of the participant’s responses to the HVFQI. To operationalize this, every categorical response label is mapped to a numeric value.Never maps to score 1, Rarely to 2, Sometimes to 3, Often to 4, and Always to 5. Not Applicable responses are filtered out (see Chandna et al., 2021 for methodology). The HVFD Severity Index is bounded between 1 and 5, with both values inclusive. Once the Severity Index is computed for every participant in the group, the distribution of Severity Indices for CVI group is compared with the distribution of the Severity Index for the NT group. The two distributions are statistically compared using the Mann–Whitney U-test and rendered as box-and-whisker plots for visual comparison.
3.1.1.2 ResultsFigure 1 shows the box-and-whiskers plots for the HVFD Severity Index distributions in the NT and CVI groups. It is evident from the figure that the two distributions are markedly different; HVFD Severity Indices for neurotypical children are substantially lower than those of the children with CVI. The Mann–Whitney U-test comparing the two distributions provides a p-value <0.00001, thus, confirming the difference in the HVFD Severity Index distributions of the two groups as statistically significant.
Figure 1. Box-and-whiskers representation of the distribution of HVFD Severity Index for group NT (4- to 18-year-old neurotypical children, n = 120) and group CVI (4- to 18-year old children with CVI, n = 92). Mann–Whitney U-test comparing the two distribution reports p-value << 0.00001.
The median HVFD Severity Index for the NT group is 1.27 while that for the CVI group is 3.23, illustrating that the children with CVI have noticeably more severe HVFDs than neurotypical children. The other two quartiles (25 and 75% of the group) exhibit similar trends where the quartile values of HVFD Severity Index for the CVI group is substantially higher than that of the NT group.
3.1.2 Spectrum of HVFDs 3.1.2.1 MethodologyTo study the breadth of the deficits that a participant reports, we introduce a new metric: HVFD Spectrum Index, which is defined as the ratio of the number of responses with Sometimes or higher response category (i.e., Sometimes, Often, Always) and the total number of responses for that participant. For example, a participant who has responded to 30 questions on the HVFQI with Sometimes, Often, or Always, and to the remaining 21 questions with Never or Rarely, the HVFD Spectrum Index will be 30/51 = 0.588. The HVFD Spectrum Index is bounded between 0 and 1. The NA responses were accounted for (Chandna et al., 2021).
Using this new metric, we compare the two study groups by first computing the HVFD Spectrum Index for every participant in the two groups, and then computing the five-point summary of the resulting distributions of HVFD Spectrum Indices for NT and CVI groups using box-and-whiskers plots.
3.1.2.2 ResultsFigure 2 shows the corresponding box-and-whiskers plots. The distribution of the HVFD Spectrum indices for the NT group is markedly distinct from that of the CVI group, which is confirmed by the Mann–Whitney U-test result with a p-value <0.00001. The CVI group was observed to have a much wider spectrum of deficits than the NT group participants. As indicated by the median values, half of the children with CVI are reporting deficits on 74% of the questions, while half of the neurotypical children are reporting deficits on 6% of the questions on the HVFQI. On average, the CVI group is reporting HVFDs for 65% of the questions while the NT group is reporting only them only for 13% of the questions (shown by cross marks in the box plots).
Figure 2. Box-and-whiskers representation of the distribution of HVFD Spectrum Index for group NT (4- to 18-year-old neurotypical children, n = 120) and group CVI (4- to 18-year-old children with CVI, n = 92). Mann–Whitney U-test comparing the two distribution reports p-value << 0.00001.
Among the NT group, seven participants reported unusually high HVFD Spectrum Indices ranging from 4.7 to 8.1 (shown in Figure 2 as six points; two datapoints overlap at 8.1). These outliers are the same individuals as previously discussed with high HVFD Severity Indices and may include participants with undiagnosed CVI. The CVI cohort has five participants (four overlapping at 0.0) that report uncharacteristically low HVFD Spectrum Indices; these are the same individuals that responded with low HVFD Severity Indices and the previous justification discussion applies.
3.1.3 Joint analysis of severity and spectrum of HVFDs 3.1.3.1 MethodologyIn this analysis, we study the relationship between the spectrum and severity of HVFDs to develop a more comprehensive picture of CVI. We use the simple tool of scatter plot for this analysis where each participant is represented by their HVFD Severity Index (X-axis) and Spectrum Index (Y-axis). The participants from the two groups, CVI and neurotypical, are distinguished on the scatter plot with datapoints displayed in different colors.
3.1.3.2 ResultsThe scatter plot in Figure 3 shows that the majority of the datapoints that correspond to neurotypical children are concentrated in the lower left section of the plot (low severity and low spectrum indices), while those for CVI children are in the top right section (high severity and high spectrum indices).
Figure 3. Scatter plot of HVFD Severity Index versus HVFD Spectrum Index for group NT (4- to 18-year old neurotypical children, n = 120) and group CVI (4- to 18-year-old children with CVI, n = 92).
It is worth noting that for the NT group (excluding the outliers), the spread along the spectrum dimension is wider (0 to 0.6, which is 60% of the range of spectrum metric) than that along the Severity Index (1.0 to 2.0, which is 25% of the range of severity metric). This again suggests that there may be participants with undiagnosed CVI type of HVFDs in the neurotypical group (Williams et al., 2021).
When comparing the two groups, NT and CVI, in terms of the spread of their indices on both metrics, the consistent trend is that the dispersion is wider for CVI group than for NT group, thus illustrating the extent of variability of symptoms in the CVI group, quantitatively supporting the heterogeneity of visual deficits in CVI, and underscoring the need for thorough HVFD profiling.
The analyses in the previous sections (Sections 3.1.1, 3.1.2, and 3.1.3) conclusively establish that the higher visual functions among children with CVI exhibit distinctly higher severity and wider spectrum of deficits than those among neurotypical children when using HVFQI questionnaire to collect this information.
3.1.4 Distinguishing CVI from NT participants at different severity levels 3.1.4.1 MethodologyThis next data analysis investigates if the HVFQI questionnaire can distinguish NTs from CVI with different severities of HVFDs. We adopt the same experimental methodology as Chandna et al. (2021) for this analysis where the CVI group is compared with the neurotypical group at each of the four response levels: Rarely, Sometimes, Often, and Always. Specifically, for a given response level, the ratio of the number of questions with responses equal or higher than that level and the total number of questions is computed for each participant. We refer to this ratio as response relative frequency (RRF). For example, RRF@Rarely+ metric for a participant who has responded to 49 out of the 51 questions with Rarely or higher response will be 0.960 (49/51); similarly, RRF@Often+ for a participant who has responded to 19 out of 51 questions with Often or Always would be 0.373 (19/51). The RRF metric is bounded between 0 and 1. We computed the four RRF values, Rarely+, Sometimes+,Often+, and Always, for each participant in both CVI and NT study groups.
3.1.4.2 ResultsFigure 4 shows the box-and-whiskers plots comparing the two resulting distributions for CVI and NT at each response level. The plots were created as described in Chandna et al. (2021), updated with the inclusion of children with visual acuity impairments ranging from mild to blind as seen in Table 3 (the previous study focused on normal visual acuity). We draw the following observations from these results.
Figure 4. Response relative frequency (RRF) at all dichotomies (Rarely+, Sometimes+, Often+, and Always+) group CVI (children with CVI, n = 92) are separate from group typical (neurotypical children, n = 120) shown as a box-and-whiskers plot (MWU p-value ≪ 0.00001).
1. The effectiveness of the HVFQI questionnaire in distinguishing children with CVI from neurotypical children holds irrespective of the severity of their higher visual function deficits.
2. At each severity level, the RRF values for the CVI group are markedly higher than those for the NT group. The Mann–Whitney U-test formally compares the distributions for all four severity levels and reports p-value << 0.00001, thus establishing the statistical significance of the differences.
3.1.5 Question-level analysis of HVFD severity 3.1.5.1 MethodologyThis analysis zooms in on individual questions to compare the two groups, CVI and NT, in terms of their HVFD severity. For each of the 51 questions on the HVFQI questionnaire, the arithmetic mean (and one standard deviation) of all the responses (severity) by the CVI group participants is computed. The same process is repeated for the participants from the neurotypical group. These question-level arithmetic means and standard deviations for the two groups are then plotted to facilitate comparative analysis.
3.1.5.2 ResultsA consistent trend across all 51 questions is evident in Figure 5 where the response means for the CVI group are distinctly higher than the response means of the neurotypical group. For some questions, for example, Q7, Q8, and Q23, the standard deviation bars for the two groups show a higher overlap. Our age range was 4–18 years and these responses may be affected by developmental age and indicate the presence of CVI traits in the NT population (Williams et al., 2021).
Figure 5. Question-level HVFD severity. Arithmetic mean and one standard deviation of participant responses for each of the 51 questions for group CVI (children with CVI, n = 92) and group neurotypical (neurotypical children, n = 120). Treating each question independently, mean MWU p-value <0.00001 for CVI and NT response distributions.
3.1.6 Question-level analysis of HVFD prevalence 3.1.6.1 MethodologyThis analysis also operates at the individual-question level but with a focus on comparing the prevalence of each deficit across the two study groups. Specifically, for each of the 51 questions, the percentage of participants that selected Sometimes, Often, or Always is computed for each group and then compared using a bar plot (see Figure 6).
Figure 6. Question-level HVFD prevalence. Percentage of participants with sometimes or higher response in the neurotypical group (children with CVI, n = 92) and in the CVI group (neurotypical children, n = 120).
3.1.6.2 ResultsThese results also demonstrate stark differences in the trends exhibited by the two study groups. For every question, substantially larger percentage of participants from the CVI group responded with presence of that deficit than participants from the NT group. For Q1, 88% of CVI children responded Sometimes or higher, while for neurotypical children that was only 34%. One of the largest differences between the two groups is observed for Q2 (4% for NT vs. 78% for CVI), and the smallest difference for Q49 (4% for NT vs. 22% for CVI).
Overall, the results from these data analyses confirm the efficacy of the HVFQI questionnaire in eliciting, quantifying, and characterizing the higher visual function deficits experienced by children with CVI. Consequentially, these analyses also demonstrate that the HVFQI questionnaire can be used effectively to distinguish children with CVI experiencing HVFDs from neurotypical children.
3.2 Objective 2: examine the relationship between visual acuity and HVFDs in children with CVIWhether and how visual acuity (VA) and HVFDs in children with CVI are related is a long-debated question in the field. So, the findings from an investigation into this question are likely to have a broad impact on CVI diagnosis, research, and practice. VA and HVFD have been directly investigated for normal visual acuity (Chandna et al., 2021) but not for different levels of VA and HVFDs. However, the relationship between visual function (VA) and functional vision (HVFD) has been questioned in low vision (Colenbrander, 2003; Colenbrander, 2010a) and in CVI (Morelli et al., 2022; Kran et al., 2019). A systematic assessment of this relationship is lacking. Here we directly examine whether HVFDs are related to the level of VA loss both for the severity (2a) and the spectrum (2b) of HVFDs.
3.2.1 DataOut of the 92 children with CVI, the VA measurements were available for 83 children. The remaining nine children could not be assessed on a logMAR VA chart due to cognitive reasons.
3.2.2 MethodologyGiven the importance of this study, we employ three statistical tests with slightly different properties and strengths to investigate the relationship between VA and HFVDs in CVI thoroughly. Specifically, we employ: (1) Fisher’s exact test (FET); (2) d-variable Hilbert–Schmidt independence criterion (dHSIC) (Pfister et al., 2018); and (3) Kendall’s rank correlation (Tau-c), each of which is described next.
(1) FET is a widely used and well-understood test, but requires us to convert the VA values and HVFD scores to categorical data. The VA values are mapped to five-category data using the vision criteria established by the World Health Organization (2019) (see Table 3).
For HVFDs, the Severity Index is employed for this analysis. This index is mapped to low or high severity category, depending on whether the index value is below or above the average severity value.
The VA and HVFD categories together create a 2 × 5 contingency matrix (two categories of HVFD severity and five categories of VA levels), where each participant is assigned to one of the 10 cells in the matrix. Table 4 presents this contingency matrix for our data.
Table 4. 2×5 Contingency matrix for Fisher’s exact test on HVFD severity (rows) vs VA category (columns).
The null hypothesis for the Fisher’s test is that the two populations, participants with low HVFD severity, and participants with high HVFD severity are equally likely to have different VA levels. Thus, the alternative hypothesis is that the two populations are not equally likely to have different VA levels.
(2) The second statistical test that we employ to investigate the relationship between VA and HVFDs is a more recent and rigorous test that eliminates the need to convert the VA and HVFD scores to categorical data. dHSIC directly determines dependence between two vectors: the VA value and HVFD Severity Index without the need to choose thresholds to create a contingency matrix, thus avoiding the loss of information that occurs due to thresholding or categorizing. The dHSIC test poses the null hypothesis that all variables are jointly independent. (In our setup, since we are testing only two variables, VA and HVFD Severity, joint independence is equivalent to pairwise independence.) The dHSIC test returns two key numbers: test statistic and critical value. The null hypothesis is rejected if the test statistic is strictly greater than critical value (that is, the two values cannot be equal).
Finally, the third test, Kendall’s rank correlation is used to test (1) the directionality of the relationship (i.e., positive, inverse, no correlation) and (2) the strength of the relationship (i.e., perfect, moderate, or weak correlation) between two ordinal variables such as the LogMAR VA values and the HVFD severity scores. In general, Kendall’s correlation coefficient (tau) is 1 (highest) when the rankings of the two variables have perfect agreement, and −1 (lowest) when the rankings of the two variables have perfect disagreement. If the two variables are independent, then Kendall’s coefficient is zero.
3.2.3 Results(1) The Fisher’s exact test on our data gives a p-value of 0.202, that is, it fails to reject the null hypothesis. Loosely speaking, this test result suggests that the two populations, low HVFD severity and high HVFD severity, are equally likely to have different VA levels. In other words, the data does not exhibit any relation between the severity of higher visual function deficits and the VA levels.
(2) The results of the dHSIC test on our data are the test statistic is 0.306 and the critical value is 0.594. Since the test results show that the test statistic is not strictly greater than critical value, the dHSIC test fails to reject the null hypothesis for our data. That is, it suggests retaining the null hypothesis—the VA and HVFDs severity are jointly independent.
(3) The Kendall’s rank correlation test on our data gives the Kendall coefficient (tau) of −0.099, demonstrating a clear lack of correlation between VA levels and HVFD severity. In fact, the near-zero tau value suggests that the two variables are independent. The corresponding p-value returned by the test (p-value = 0.217) is too large to reject the null hypothesis, thus retaining the null hypothesis that VA levels and HVFD severity are independent.
Figure 7A shows a scatterplot of the VA and the HVFD Severity Index, which visually illustrates a general lack of correlation between the two factors. Figure 7B compares the HVFD Spectrum Index with VA values, and similar trends are evident with this metric as well.
Figure 7. Scatter plot of VA versus HVFD for children with CVI (n = 83). (A) VA vs. HVFD Severity Index. (B) VA vs. HVFD Spectrum Index.
Overall, we see a clear consensus between all three tests in demonstrating that the wide spectrum of HVFDs elicited by the HVFQI are not independent of VA.
3.3 Objective 3: study the relationship between age and HVFDs in children with CVIThe relationship between HVFDs and age has not yet been broadly characterized. Previous studies suggest that visual function improves with age among children with CVI due to physiological visual processing development and neuroplasticity (Bennett et al., 2020; Galli et al., 2022; Watson et al., 2007; Matsuba and Jan, 2006; Lim et al., 2005). This indicates that there is a negative correlation between age and relative visual function deficit severity, but HVFs were not studied. We doubt if there is a relationship between VA and HVFDs with varying age and attempt an initial investigation of age and absolute severity and spectrum of HVFDs in our cross-sectional cohort and suggest further studies.
For this investigation, we employ the same two statistical tests that we employed earlier: (i) dHSIC to test for independence between age and HVFDs and (ii) Kendall’s rank correlation to determine if there is an ordinal association between HVFDs and age. We have to omit Fisher’s exact test because it requires categorical data and there are no conventions for mapping the 4- to 18-year age range to categories.
3.3.1 DataNinety two children with established diagnosis of CVI (group CVI).
3.3.2 ResultsThe results from this preliminary analysis are as follows:
1) For the dHSIC test, the null hypothesis is that age and HVFD Severity are jointly independent. The results from the dHSIC test are as follows: test statistic = 0.791; critical value = 0.560. Since the test statistic is strictly greater than the critical value, the dHSIC test rejects the null hypothesis. Thus, age and HVFD severity are not independent as per the dHSIC test.
2) The Kendall’s rank correlation test provides a tau value of 0.123, which suggests a weak positive correlation between age and HVFD severity. However, the corresponding p-value is 0.102, which is too high and in the context of correlation analysis indicates that the observed correlation between age and HVFD severity is by chance, that is, the correlation is really 0.
These contradictory results underscore the preliminary nature of this investigation. A larger study—longitudinal and controls for factors such as the onset age, developmental environments, and habilitations—is part of our near-term future study plan.
Figure 8A shows a scatterplot of age and HVFD severity index, which visually illustrates a general lack of correlation between the two factors. Figure 8B compares HVFD Spectrum Index with age, and similar trends are evident with this metric as well.
Figure 8. Scatter plot of age versus HVFD for children with CVI (n = 92). (A) Age vs. HVFD Severity Index. (B) Age vs. HVFD Spectrum Index.
Overall, the analysis hints at a relationship between participant’s age and their higher visual function deficits; however, larger studies need to be undertaken to conclusively establish the strength and polarity of this relationship.
4 DiscussionOur overarching goal behind the three-pronged investigation, described in the prior sections, is to deepen our understanding of the higher visual function deficits in children with CVI and the relationship between HVFDs and visual acuity. Overall, our findings and those of previous studies examining VA and HVFDs (van Genderen et al., 2012; Chandna et al., 2021) provide substantial evidence to support the integration of HVFQI into clinical and habilitational practice for children with CVI regardless of VA measures, where applicable.
4.1 The HVFQI is effective in eliciting HVFDs in children with CVIOur first significant finding confirms and adds to our previous research of the ability of HVFQI to elicit HVFDs, showing Likert scale scores that are clearly distinct (higher) from those of neurotypical participants (Figure 4). This is important for establishing the HVFQI as an effective clinical tool. That being said, the instrument is not perfect and the following discussion on error analysis and possible contributing factors provide further insights for future improvement.
As seen in Figure 1, both the participant groups have outliers—there are seven participants in the NT group for whom the HVFD Severity Index is higher than the expected variation in NT group’s distribution (blue dots), and five participants in the CVI group with Severity Index lower than the expected variation in CVI group’s distribution (orange dots; 4 overlapping at 0.0).
We suspect that the seven outliers in the NT group are participants with false-negative cases with HVFD traits suggestive of CVI. The basis for our suspicion is the result from a recent study by Williams et al. (2021) where they observed that one in 30 children in the primary school population have CVI traits of HVFDs presumably from undiagnosed CVI. This result confirms their recommendation for further investigation into the problem of underdiagnosis of CVI among children.
The five outliers in the CVI group with scores typical of NT group present a more complicated scenario because there are several possible factors at play here:
1. Participants incorrectly answered the questions. Two of the participants with CVI diagnosis chose Never for all the questions. In our study, the interviewer asked the question but refrained from influencing the response. Survey fatigue might also be a contributing factor (Gibson and Bowling, 2019; Tourangeau et al., 2010).
2. Participants have such severe developmental disabilities that they may be incorrectly diagnosed as CVI (false-positive CVI cases). Four participants out of the five outliers had a history of significant brain-based comorbidity conditions. The fifth child was one of the triplets with very low birth weight and had suffered traumatic brain injury (classified as mild). Prematurity and low birth weight have been associated with CVI behaviorisms (Dutton, 2013; Macintyre-Béon et al., 2013; Slidsborg et al., 2012; Geldof et al., 2015) as is mild traumatic brain injury (Rauchman et al., 2022; Brosseau-Lachaine et al., 2008). However, this may be because of other masking comorbidities and severe developmental disabilities (Jimenez-Gomez et al., 2022).
3. Participants have adapted to HVFDs. Children often develop efficient adaptations to compensate for deficits of various nature including ocular, which in the current scenario can mask the observation of HVFDs (Zihl and Dutton, 2015; Ortibus et al., 2019).
These observations about the outliers suggest further research into questionnaire administration methodology and into the development of shorter questionnaire and screeners (Chandna et al., 2021). Studies to develop tools and tests that can better distinguish CVI traits from common comorbidities are also needed. Finally, NT outliers found to have HVFD traits on the HVFQI may need further assessment to determine whether they have CVI. The outliers in the CVI group should have further assessments, for example, to determine whether they have developed adaptations that mask the underlying HVFDs.
While this study confirms the efficacy of the HVFQI for quantitative characterization of HVFDs as demonstrated in Chandna et al. (2021), it goes beyond just that finding. We add quantitative measures of the spectrum and severity index for each participant.
4.1.1 HVFD severity index and spectrum indexThe ability of HVFQI to capture and quantify both the spectrum and the severity of HVFDs for each child is powerful. For clinicians, the indices and the information about HVFD spectrum and severity are crucial for the following reasons:
(1) These measures help understand HVFDs by viewing a 2-D picture as they affect the individual child. For example, a child with CVI may have a narrow spectrum in the presence of high severity distinct from another child with a broad spectrum with low severity. Considered together, this 2-D view would need a different (re)habilitational approach for the two children.
(2) Longitudinal measures of spectrum and severity scores and the changes in the Likert response scale for the HVFDs would help monitor the efficacy of intervention and, if necessary, indicate a modification of strategies.
(3) These measures would provide practical information to the family, school, and child and response to therapy.
4.2 HVFDs are independent of VA lossOur third significant finding from this investigation, taken together with our previous results, provides conclusive evidence for the independence of HVFDs from visual acuity measures. Previous study had studied the effectiveness of HVFQI only for children with normal VA. Our findings substantially expand the applicability of the HVFQI for identifying HVFDs in children with reduced levels of VA (normal to 1.0 Log MAR VA) for both spectrum and severity of HVFD. This finding should enable us to make substantial progress toward including HVFD characterization in the following: (a) a more comprehensive definition of CVI; (b) emerging diagnostic criteria; (c) the spectrum and severity in the description of CVI; and (d) International Classification of Diseases (ICD) coding for the description of CVI (to accurately reflect the criteria for the definition of CVI). The independence of HVFDs from VA measures also means that for children and families, (re)habilitational measures should include accommodation and intervention strategies that are aimed toward HVFDs, including a multisensory approach and not entirely dependent on the strategies for improvements in visual acuity.
What possible model could explain th
Comments (0)