Influencing the Insulin System by Placebo Effects in Patients With Diabetes Type 2 and Healthy Controls: A Randomized Controlled Trial

INTRODUCTION

Placebo effects are positive treatment outcomes that cannot be attributed to the pharmacological mechanisms of the treatment but are caused by the psychosocial context (1). Placebo effects can be induced by positive patient-doctor communication, observational learning, or associative leaning through classical conditioning procedures. Accumulating evidence suggests that it is possible to modulate endocrine functions using classical conditioning (2,3): coupling of an active medication (unconditioned stimulus [US]) with an initially neutral stimulus (conditioned stimulus [CS]). In case of endocrine conditioning, hormonal-stimulating or inhibiting medication (US) gets associated with the CS, and later, the mere presentation of the CS alone leads to changes in hormone levels or triggers effects associated with this hormone.

Several possible clinical applications of placebo effects induced by conditioning were proposed (4,5). For example, dosages of standard treatments can be reduced using placebo-controlled dose reduction protocols, in which an active drug gets pharmacologically conditioned and then a part of it is replaced by a placebo while maintaining the efficacy of treatment (4). Placebo effects can also boost the efficiency of treatments when conditioning procedures are added to the standard treatment protocols (6).

The most convincing evidence for endocrine conditioning comes from studies on conditioning of insulin and glucose responses in animals and healthy humans (7–11). Insulin and glucose responses seem to be particularly malleable by the mechanisms of conditioning (12), probably because of their acute homeostatic functions aimed at maintaining glucose metabolism. Cephalic phase release of insulin, for example, is a transient pulse of insulin that has been observed in both animals and humans in response to food cues, such as the smell of food, or the time of the day when food is regularly taken (12). This conditioned response seems to help prepare the organism for the upcoming homeostatic changes related to the food consumption and prevent hyperglycemia caused by consumption of large amounts of food (13). Not only naturally occurring associations, such as associations between the smell of food and food intake, can trigger conditioned insulin responses. Experimental studies demonstrated that coupling of food with any neutral stimuli, such as a sound or a light, can trigger conditioned insulin release (14–16). Moreover, insulin and glucose responses can be conditioned using US other than food. Using insulin injections as a US, it was found possible to classically condition glucose decrease in healthy young volunteers (9,10). Another study successfully conditioned insulin release and glucose decrease in healthy volunteers using intranasal insulin administration as a US (7).

Up-to-date, most of the animal research on insulin conditioning has been done in male mice or rats (for the review, see Ref. (2)), and the few available human studies were performed in young male volunteers. Therefore, it remains unknown whether sex or age might play any role in the conditionability of insulin effects. Importantly, there are no reports of the possibility to condition insulin responses in metabolic disorders. Particularly, patients with diabetes type 2 might benefit from conditioning with intranasal insulin as an US because intranasal insulin has been shown to have a number of benefits for patients with diabetes type 2. Conditioning with insulin might trigger conditioned insulin release and glucose decrease (7) without causing common adverse effects of intravenous insulin injections such as hypoglycemia and hypertension (17). Moreover, because intranasal insulin normalizes hypothalamic neuronal activity in response to glucose ingestion, it could be especially favorable for type 2 diabetes patients who demonstrate distorted brain responses to glucose (18,19). In addition, evidence suggests that intranasal insulin decreases food intake and hunger (20,21) and improves memory both in healthy volunteers and patients with diabetes type 2 (22,23). Taken together, classical conditioning with intranasal insulin has a wide range of potential positive effects for patients with diabetes type 2.

The aim of the present study was to investigate the effects of conditioning with intranasal insulin on blood glucose, insulin, C-peptide, hunger, and memory in a group of patients with diabetes type 2 and age- and sex-matched healthy controls. In addition, we aimed to explore differences between healthy individuals and patients with diabetes type 2, as their responses to insulin and conditioning might differ because of insulin resistance (24) or different baseline levels of glucose (25) or metabolic hormones (26,27). Finally, we explored possible sex differences in the effects of conditioning with intranasal insulin.

METHODS Participants

Patients diagnosed with diabetes type 2 and healthy controls were included in the study. Healthy controls were matched for age (the mean age of the groups was matched ±1 year) and sex to the patients’ group. Inclusion criteria for the patients were as follows: a) being older than 18 years, b) current diagnosis of diabetes type 2, and c) taking metformin and/or participating in a lifestyle intervention (e.g., diet) to control their diabetes. Exclusion criteria for both healthy subjects and patients were as follows: a) use of insulin or insulin stimulating medications; b) use of medication that influences glucose metabolism (e.g., corticosteroid medication, chemotherapy, β-blockers); c) diagnosis of a chronic noncommunicable disease (degenerative diseases, malignant neoplasms such as cancer, diabetes type 1, autoimmune diseases); d) diagnosis of an acute infectious disease (such as meningitis, hepatitis B, and bacterial pneumonia); e) current diagnosis of a mental disorder; f) chronic and/or acute rhinitis; g) anatomic deviations of the nose; h) substance abuse (e.g., drugs or alcohol); and i) pregnancy.

The sample size calculation was based on the results of the study with a comparable design in healthy participants, which reported an effect size of d = 0.77 (7). A power analysis using this effect size yielded that 16 participants per condition and per group are needed with a power of 0.8 and a two-sided α of .05 as determined by G*Power software.

Study Design

The study had a double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled design. Thirty-two patients with diabetes type 2 and 32 healthy controls were randomized to one of two groups in a double-blind manner: a) conditioned group and b) control group. Men and women were equally distributed between the groups. This study was an adaptation of the study design used by Stockhorst and colleagues (7) for conditioning insulin responses in healthy participants. The study conditions are presented in Figure S1, Supplemental Digital Content, https://links.lww.com/PSYMED/A936.

The study was approved by the Medical Ethical Committee of Leiden, Den Haag, Delft, under protocol number P18.222.

Randomization was performed by the Department of Clinical Pharmacy of the Leiden University Medical Center. A block randomization was used with a size of eight participants per block. Equal numbers of men and women were randomized to each condition. The pharmacy was responsible for assigning participants to the conditions. The experimenter was blinded regarding the conditions, and the list coupling participants numbers with conditions remained at the pharmacy until the last participant was tested.

Procedure

The data collection was done from May 2019 to March 2021. The study procedures are presented in Figure S2, Supplemental Digital Content, https://links.lww.com/PSYMED/A937. Candidates who expressed their interest to participate in the study were first contacted by telephone for an initial screening during which the inclusion criteria were checked and participants were provided with study details. Participants were informed that the study aimed to investigate the effects of intranasal insulin on several blood measures, hunger, and memory. They remained unaware of the specific conditioning hypothesis.

Eligible participants were invited to the laboratory of the Clinical Research Unit of the Leiden University Medical Center for two visits. They were asked to refrain from eating, drinking alcohol and caffeinated drinks, and exercising for a minimum of 12 hours before the study. Patients who received metformin as a treatment were asked not to take it the morning of the study, but they were allowed to take it immediately after the end of the session.

On day 1, upon arrival to the laboratory, participants signed an informed consent form. Their weight and height were measured, and their health status and medication use were assessed. After that, an intravenous catheter was inserted into the median cubital vein by a licensed nurse followed by a baseline blood draw immediately after. Subsequently, participants were asked to smell a fragrant pen for 1 minute by holding the pen approximately 1 cm away from their nose. Immediately thereafter, participants in the conditioned group received 20 units of intranasal insulin spray into one nostril with one puff. Participants in the control group received a placebo spray. Right after administration of the spray, participants were asked to smell the fragrant pen for 1 more minute. Afterward, another sample of blood was drawn. After the blood draw, participants were asked to rate how well they could smell the odor, and their hunger was measured. This procedure of smell-spray-smell administration followed by blood draw and hunger rating was repeated six times every 15 minutes. In between, participants could read a newspaper. After the last spray, participants were given the first part of the memory task. Fifteen minutes after the last spray, the last blood sample was drawn and the catheter was removed. Subsequently, the second part of the memory task was done followed by a mobile food Approach Avoidance Task and a bogus taste test. Day 2 was identical to day 1; however, participants in both conditioned and control groups received a placebo nasal spray. At the end of day 2, participants were fully debriefed about the aims of the study and received a reward of 100 euros.

Materials Unconditioned Stimulus

The US was 20 units (0.2 ml) of fast-acting insulin (Insulin NovoRapid; Novo Nordisk), administered with the MAD Nasal Intranasal Mucosal Atomization Device (Teleflex) by a trained member of the research team. Six administrations of insulin were done on day 1 in the conditioned group with a break of 15 minutes between the administrations. The spray was administered alternating between the left and then right nostrils. The same dosage of insulin has been successfully used in previous research on insulin conditioning in healthy volunteers (7).

Placebo nasal spray was used in the control group on day 1 and day 2 and in the conditioned group on day 2. The spray was prepared by the Department of Clinical Pharmacy of the Leiden University Medical Center. Because of unavailability of meta-cresol, the preservative that gives a particular smell to the insulin nasal spray, another preservative, chlorobutanol, was used to add a smell to the placebo.

Conditioned Stimulus

A smell of rosewood oil was used as a CS. The oil was purchased online from www.aromaolie.nl. This aroma oil has previously been used successfully in a study on classical conditioning of oxytocin (28) by our study group, and, mixed with peppermint oils, in previous research on conditioning of insulin responses (7,9). This smell was rated as pleasant but unfamiliar in previous research (28). Commercially available felt-tip pens were filled with rosewood oil used as a CS. During the smell presentation, participants were asked to hold the pen at approximately 1 cm in front of both nostrils for 1 minute before and 1 minute after the nose spray administration.

Measurements

Glucose, insulin, and C-peptide levels were measured in blood at baseline, after each spray administration, and 15 minutes after the last spray.

Hunger was measured with a self-rated question, “How hungry do you feel at the moment.” Participants were asked to give an answer on an 11-point numeric rating scale (0, “not hungry at all”; 10, “the worst hunger I have ever experienced”). Hunger was measured at the beginning of each session, 5 minutes after each spray administration, and 20 minutes after the last spray administration.

Approach tendencies toward food were measured at the end of each day with a validated mobile phone approach avoidance task in which participants were presented pictures of food and nonfood objects (29). The task consisted of two blocks: in the congruent block, participants were asked to approach foods by pulling them toward themselves and to avoid objects by pushing them away. In the incongruent block, they were asked to do the opposite—to avoid foods and to approach objects. During each movement, reaction times and response forces were measured. Food approach tendencies are calculated by comparing how fast/strong participants approach foods compared with avoiding them. In total, 80 photographs of food and 40 photographs of objects were presented in a randomized order. During each response, the telephone tracked the gravity- and rotation-corrected acceleration of the movement in the direction perpendicular to the face of the screen (100-Hz sampling rate). Based on this acceleration, two outcome measures were calculated: reaction times (the time between the stimulus presentation and start of response) and force (peak acceleration, in meters per second squared) (29). The pictures for the task were taken from the Food Pics Database (30). The task was presented to the participants on both day 1 and day 2 after the last blood draw.

Food consumption was measured with a taste test adapted from previous studies (31,32). At the end of days 1 and 2, participants were offered several snacks: nuts, cucumbers, blueberries, tomatoes, red pepper, and carrots. They could eat as much as they wanted to. Afterward, the weight of the eaten snacks was measured and the total number of calories eaten was calculated.

Memory was assessed by the Auditory Verbal Learning Test in which 15 words were read to participants 5 times, and participants were asked to repeat all the words they could remember after each reading. Fifteen minutes after the first assessment, participants were asked to name the words they still were able to recall. This is a reliable test for measuring learning and memory (33). Immediate recall scores were calculated by summing the number of all correctly recalled words during the first five assessments. Learning scores were calculated by subtracting the number of the words successfully recalled on the first assessment from the number of the words recalled during the fifth assessment. Percent of forgetting scores was calculated by subtracting the number of words recalled on the delayed recall task from the number of words recalled on the fifth assessment. Version A of the task was given to participants after the last spray administration on day 1 and version B of the task after the last spray administration on day 2.

Perceived group allocation was measured at the end of day 2. Participants were asked to indicate whether they think they received insulin or placebo spray on each of the experimental days.

Statistical Analysis

The data analyses were performed using SPSS Statistics version 21 (IBM Corporation, Armonk, New York) and RStudio (version 1.1.447; R version 4.0.4). All analyses were performed with a two-tailed significance level of α < .05. The data and all analyses codes are available on Open Science Framework (osf.io/nywhq).

A 2 condition (conditioned versus control) × 2 group (healthy versus patient) multivariate analysis of variance was used to compare the groups on the baseline characteristics: age, body mass index, baseline glucose, insulin and C-peptide values, and baseline hunger.

The lmer function of the nlme package in R (R Core Team, 2013) was used for the liner mixed-effects model analyses. Mixed-effects models were applied to the data that included repeated measures (glucose, insulin, C-peptide, hunger, and approach-avoidance task). In all models, the intercept was allowed to vary randomly across participants.

The multilevel structure of the data was defined by measurement time (level 1) nested in participants (level 2). Parameters were estimated using the full-maximum likelihood procedure. In all models, the intercept was allowed to vary randomly across participants. Random slopes did not improve the fit of the models, and therefore, they were removed from the final analysis. The assumption of linearity was checked for each model by plotting the model residuals versus the predictor, and visually inspecting the plots. Homogeneity of variance was checked by the Levene test. Each model was also checked for the normal distribution of its residuals by looking at QQ plots created with Lattice package. In case of violation of any of the assumptions, the data were transformed. The following variables were transformed because of the violation of the homogeneity of variance and nonnormal distribution of the residuals: logarithmic transformation was applied to glucose levels on day 2 and C-peptide levels on day 1 and day 2, the square root transformation was applied to the insulin levels on day 2, and inversion transformation was applied to the reaction time in the approach-avoidance task.

To examine the effects of intranasal insulin administration on blood glucose levels on day 1, a mixed model was performed with day 1 glucose levels as a dependent variable, condition (conditioned versus control), group (healthy versus patient), measurement time (0, 15, 30, 45, 60, 75, or 90 minutes after the first spray administration), baseline glucose levels (measured before the first spray administration), and the interactions between these variables as predictors. To examine the effects of conditioning on blood glucose levels, the same mixed-models analysis was performed but with the measures of day 2. The same analyses were run with insulin, C-peptide, and hunger for each day separately to investigate whether intranasal insulin and conditioning affected these measures. In case an interaction factor was significant, separate models were run for either two groups (healthy and patients) or conditions (conditioned and control), depending on which of the factors was included in this interaction. All mixed models were repeated with sex as a predictor in an exploratory analysis to investigate whether sex affected the relationships between the variables. The effect sizes (Cohen d) of all linear mixed-effects models were calculated with EMAtools package. Cohen d = 0.2 was interpreted as a small effect size, d = 0.5 as a medium effect size, and d = 0.8 as a large effect size.

To examine whether intranasal insulin and conditioning affected the approach tendencies toward food, two mixed models were performed. The first model included condition (conditioned versus control), groups (patient versus healthy), day (1 versus 2), stimulus type (food versus object), movement type (pull versus push), and the interaction between these factors as predictors and reaction time as a dependent variable. The second model included the same predictors but movement force as a dependent variable.

A 2 condition (conditioned versus control) × 2 group (healthy versus patient) factorial analysis of variance was used to compare the groups on food consumption during the bogus test: analyses were run separately for day 1 and day 2 with calories eaten as an outcome measure.

A 2 condition (conditioned versus control) × 2 group (healthy versus patient) factorial analysis of variance was used to compare the groups on their memory scores (immediate recall, learning, percentage forgetting). As three separate memory outcomes were used in the analysis, Bonferroni corrections were applied and α level was set to .016.

To evaluate success of the blinding, χ2 test was performed comparing the number of successful guesses with the expected number of successful guesses.

RESULTS Participants

Thirty-two patients with diabetes type 2 (17 men, mean [standard deviation] age = 68.3 [11.86] years) and 32 healthy volunteers (17 men, mean [standard deviation] age = 67.8 [6.12] years) were included in the study. The flowchart with the numbers of screened participants and dropouts is presented in Figure S3, Supplemental Digital Content, https://links.lww.com/PSYMED/A938.

Baseline characteristics are presented in Table 1. There was no difference between conditions (conditioned group versus control) in any baseline characteristic (F(10,50) = 0.93, p = .52, Wilk Λ = 0.84). Patients had a higher body mass index (F(1,63) = 14.86, p < .001), higher baseline levels of glucose (F(1,63) = 114.32, p < .001) and C-peptide (F(1,63) = 9.87, p < .001) on day 1, higher glucose levels (F(1,63) = 91.72, p < .001), and C-peptide (F(1,63) = 4.95, p = .030) on day 2 and higher hunger at baseline on day 1 (F(1,63) = 14.61, p < .001) than healthy controls.

TABLE 1 - Baseline Characteristics and Taste Test, Approach-Avoidance Task and Memory Scores With Means, Standard Errors in Parentheses, and Number of Observations in Square Brackets Across Groups and Study Conditions Conditioned Group Control Group Patients Healthy Controls Patients Healthy Controls Age, y 68.31 (2.37) [16] 67.69 (2.37) [16] 68.20 (2.44) [16] 67.81 (5.5) [16] Body mass index, kg/m2 29.77 (0.84) [16] 25.08 (0.84) [16] 27.77 (0.87) [16] 25.92 (0.84) [16] Baseline insulin, day 1, mU/L 14.59 (2.17) [16] 9.01 (2.17) [16] 12.69 (2.17) [16] 12.07 (2.17) [16] Baseline glucose, day 1, mmol/L 8.49 (0.28) [16] 5.35 (0.28) [16] 8.36 (0.28) [16] 5.43 (0.28) [16] Baseline C-peptide, day 1, mmol/L 1.12 (0.10) [16] 0.74 (0.10) [16] 1.14 (0.10) [16] 0.90 (0.10) [16] Baseline hunger, day 1 4.5 (0.64) [16] 2.09 (0.64) [16] 5.2 (0.66) [16] 2.69 (0.64) [16] Baseline insulin, day 2, mU/L 12.8 (2.10) [16] 9.42 (2.10) [16] 10.67 (2.17) [16] 13.11 (2.10) [16] Baseline glucose, day 2, mmol/L 8.34 (0.30) [16] 5.28 (0.30) [16] 8.18 (0.31) [16] 5.41 (0.30) [16] Baseline C-peptide, day 2, mmol/L 1.08 (0.10) [16] 0.77 (0.10) [16] 1.07 (0.10) [16] 0.94 (0.10) [16] Baseline hunger, day 2 3.91 (0.60) [16] 2.59 (0.60) [16] 4.73 (0.62) [16] 4.19 (0.60) [16] Taste test day 1, kcal 74.99 (15.66) [16] 74.23 (20.56) [16] 142.26 (44.79) [16] 71.59 (17.49) [16] Taste test day 2, kcal 78.77 (19.72) [16] 89.62 (23.7) [16] 127.45 (37.58) [16] 62.47 (13.95) [16] Approach to food reaction time, day 1, s a 0.17 (0.23) [14] 0.14 (0.25) [14] 0.17 (0.31) [15] 0.25 (0.29) [15] Approach to food reaction time day 2, s a 0.21 (0.22) [14] 0.25 (0.25) [14] 0.28 (0.24) [15] 0.31 (0.21) [15] Approach to food force day 1, m/s2a 0.43 (5.96) [14] 3.93 (6.27) [14] −1.42 (6.75) [15] 0.91 (7.41) [15] Approach to food force day 2, m/s2a 2.29 (6.33) [14] 1.98 (5.95) [14] 0.56 (7.50) [15] −0.89 (8.48) [15] Immediate recall day 1 44.60 (2.74) [10] 43.00 (2.50) [12] 40.89 (2.89) [10] 40.42 (2.50) [13] Immediate recall day 2 45.20 (2.80) [11] 46.25 (2.55) [12] 40.78 (2.95) [10] 41.58 (2.55) [12] Learning day 1 5.30 (0.56) [10] 5.33 (0.51) [12] 5.67 (0.59) [10] 4.67 (0.51) [13] Learning day 2 5.90 (0.84) [11] 5.75 (0.76) [12] 4.67 (0.88) [10] 5.33 (0.76) [12] Percent forgetting day 1 0.21 (0.07) [10] 0.27 (0.06) [12] 0.269 (0.07) [10] 0.164 (0.06) [13] Percent forgetting day 2 0.24 (0.07) [11] 0.36 (0.06) [12] 0.25 (0.07) [10] 0.22 (0.06) [12]

a Difference between pull and push conditions.


Blood Glucose Effects of Insulin Spray (Day 1)

The effect of time-condition-group interaction (B = 0.03, SE = 0.02, p = .027, d = 0.23) on the blood glucose levels on day 1 was significant. Glucose levels were significantly decreasing with time in healthy participants (B = −0.02, SE = 0.01, p = .002, d = 0.46). In patients, there was a significant time-condition interaction (B = 0.03, SE = 0.01, p = .011, d = 0.37), indicating a significant decrease in glucose levels in patients who received a placebo spray, whereas this decrease was absent in patients who received insulin (Figures 1, 1.A–1.C, 2.A–2.C).

F1FIGURE 1:

Mean changes of glucose levels from baseline with standard errors.

When sex was added to the model as a predictor, a significant time-condition-group-sex interaction was found (B = 0.05, SE = 0.02, p = .025, d = 0.23). There was a significant time-condition interaction in men (B = 0.046, SE = 0.02, p = .021, d = 0.33), indicating that there was a significant decrease in blood glucose levels in men who received placebo, whereas men who received insulin had stable glucose levels (Figures 1.1.A, 1.2.A). The effect of condition (B = 0.01, SE = 0.12, p = .92, d = 0.04) and interactions between condition and other predictors (all p values > .54) were insignificant in women.

Effects of Conditioning (Day 2)

The effect of time-group interaction (B = −0.005, SE = 0.001, p = .003, d = 0.40) on glucose on day 2 was significant, indicating that there was a decrease in blood glucose levels in both healthy participants (B = −0.003, SE = 0.001, p = .008, d = 0.43) and patients (B = −0.01, SE = 0.001, p < .001, d = 0.92); however, this decrease was more pronounced in patients (Figures 1.3A, 1.4A). Condition (conditioned versus control) did not affect glucose levels on day 2 (B = −0.0004, SE = 0.02, p = .98, d = 0.07).

When sex was added to the model as a predictor, a significant effect of a time-condition-sex interaction (B = 0.001, SE = 0.0003, p = .024, d = 0.23) was found. There was a significant effect of time-condition interaction in men (B = −0.02, SE = 0.01, p = .024, d = 0.32) but not women (B = −0.001, SE = 0.03, p = .98, d = 0.09), indicating that control men had a decrease in blood glucose level, which was absent in conditioned men (Figures 1, 3.A, 3.B, 3.C, 4.A, 4.B, 4.C).

Insulin Effects of Insulin Spray (Day 1)

There was no effect of condition (insulin versus placebo spray; B = −0.07, SE = 0.15, p = .67, d = 0.11), group (B = 0.14, SE = 0.15, p = .36, d = 0.24), or time (B = −0.02, SE = 0.01, t(380) = −1.69, p = .092, d = 0.17) on insulin levels on day 1, neither was the interaction between these factors significant (B = 0.05, SE = 0.03, p = .084, d = 0.18; Figure S4, 1.a, 1.b, Supplemental Digital Content, https://links.lww.com/PSYMED/A939).

There was no significant effect of sex on insulin levels on day 1 (B = −0.01, SE = 0.21, p = .98, d = 0.01); also, the interactions of other variables with sex were not significant (all p values > .14).

Effects of Conditioning With Insulin (Day 2)

There was no effect of condition (conditioned versus control; B = 0.47, SE = 1.03, p = .65) or time (B = 0.02, SE = 0.09, p = .83, d = 0.02) on insulin levels on day 2. Patients had significantly higher insulin levels than healthy controls after controlling for baseline levels (B = 2.62, SE = 1.03, p = .014, d = 0.66; Figure S4, 2.a, 2.b, Supplemental Digital Content, https://links.lww.com/PSYMED/A939).

There was no significant effect of sex on insulin levels on day 1 (B = −0.81, SE = 1.07, p = .45, d = 0.20); also, the interactions of other variables with sex were not significant (all p values > .41).

C-Peptide Effects of Insulin Spray (Day 1)

There was a significant effect of the time-condition-group interaction on the C-peptide levels on day 1 (B = 0.01, SE = 0.001, p = .008, d = 0.27). Patients had a significant increase in C-peptide levels during the session (B = 0.01, SE = 0.002, p = .001, d = 0.47). In healthy participants, there was a significant time-condition interaction (B = −0.01, SE = 0.003, p = .006, d = 0.40), demonstrating a decrease in C-peptide levels in healthy participants who received insulin spray, and no change in healthy participants who received placebo (Figure S5, 1.a, 1.b, Supplemental Digital Content, https://links.lww.com/PSYMED/A940).

The time-condition-sex interaction was significant (B = 0.04, SE = 0.01, p < .001, d = 0.51). There was a significant time-condition-group interaction in men (B = 0.014, SE = 0.006, p = .017, d = 0.34), whereas this interaction did not reach significance in women (B = 0.007, SE = 0.004, p = .056, d = 0.29), indicating that the effect found in the whole group was influenced primary by men.

Effects of Conditioning With Insulin (Day 2)

There was no effect of condition (B = 0.05, SE = 0.05, p = .27, d = 0.29), group (B = 0.05, SE = 0.05, p = .26, d = 0.30), or time (B = 0.001, SE = 0.002, p = .83, d = 0.02) on the C-peptide levels on day 2 (Figure S5, 2.a, 2.b, Supplemental Digital Content, https://links.lww.com/PSYMED/A940). There was no effect of sex on conditioned C-peptide levels (B = −0.002, SE = 0.09, p = .98, d = 0.006), and the interactions of other variables with sex were not significant (all p values > .315).

Hunger Effects of Insulin Spray (Day 1)

There was a significant effect of time (B = 0.26, SE = 0.06, p < .001, d = 0.41) and group-time interaction (B = −0.25, SE = 0.09, p = .007, d = 0.27) on hunger levels on day 1. Hunger increased with time in healthy participants (B = 0.26, SE = 0.07, p < .001, d = 0.53) but stayed stable in patients (B = 0.01, SE = 0.06, p = .92, d = 0.02). There was no effect of condition (insulin versus placebo spray; B = −0.46, SE = 0.69, p = .50, d = 0.17). There was no effect of sex on hunger levels on day 1 (B = −0.48, SE = 0.97, p = .63, d = 0.12), the interactions of other variables with sex were also not significant (all p values > .107; Figures 2, 1.A, 1.B).

F2FIGURE 2:

Mean changes of hunger from baseline with standard errors.

Effects of Conditioning With Insulin (Day 2)

There was a significant effect of time-condition-group interaction (B = 0.31, SE = 0.09, p < .001, d = 0.35) on hunger on day 2. The time-condition interaction was significant in healthy controls (B = 0.27, SE = 0.06, p < .001, d = 0.62) but not in patients (B = 0.12, SE = 0.53, p = .82, d = 0.03), indicating that hunger increased with time in healthy controls in the control group, whereas it stayed stable in the conditioned healthy controls (Figures 2, 2.A, 2.B). When sex was added in the model, the time-condition-group-sex interaction was significant (B = −0.42, SE = 0.18, p = .011, d = 0.27). In men, group-condition-time interaction was significant (B = 0.52, SE = 0.12, p < .001, d = 0.72), indicating an increase in hunger in healthy men from the control group and stable hunger levels in healthy conditioned men and male patients. In women, neither condition (B = 0.43, SE = 0.98, p = .67, d = 0.18) nor any interactions with condition were significant (all p values > .23).

Memory and Food Approach Tendencies Effects of Insulin Spray (Day 1)

There was no effect of intranasal spray administration on the food approach tendencies (reaction time: B = −0.01, SE = 0.08, p = .93, d = 0.16; force: B = −2.89, SE = 3.09, p = .35, d = 0.18), food consumption (F(3,62) = 0.75, p = .39, η2 = 0.01), and any of the memory scores (all p values > .171). The scores are presented in Table 1, and the results of the analyses of each of the memory scores are presented in Table 2.

TABLE 2 - The Factorial ANOVAs Comparing Groups and Conditions on Memory Scores

留言 (0)

沒有登入
gif