Category Mistakes Electrified

Asher, Nicholas. 2011. Lexical meaning in context: A web of words. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Book  Google Scholar 

Baggio, Giosuè. 2018. Meaning in the Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Book  Google Scholar 

Barrett, Sarah E., and Michael D. Rugg. 1990. Event-related potentials and the semantic matching of pictures. In Brain and cognition 14 (2): 201–212.

Article  Google Scholar 

Beardsley, Monroe C. 1962. The metaphorical twist. Philosophy and phenomenological research 22 (3): 293–307.

Article  Google Scholar 

Bentin, Shlomo, Gegory McCarthy, and Charles C. Wood. 1985. Event-related potentials, lexical decision and semantic priming. Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology 60: 343–355.

Article  Google Scholar 

Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, Ina, and Matthias Schlesewsky. 2008. An alternative perspective on"semantic P600" effects in language comprehension. In Brain research reviews 59 (1): 55–73.

Brouwer, Harm, Matthew W. Crocker, Noortje J. Venhuizen, and John C. J. Hoeks. 2017. A neurocomputational model of the N400 and the P600 in language processing. In Cognitive science 41: 1318–1352.

Brouwer, Harm, Hartmut Fitz, and John Hoeks. 2012. Getting real about semantic illusions: Rethinking the functional role of the P600 in language comprehension. In Brain research 1446: 127–143.

Brown, Colin, and Peter Hagoort. 1993. The processing nature of the N400: Evidence from masked priming. In Journal of cognitive neuroscience 5 (1): 34–44.

Chomsky, Noam. 1965. Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar 

Chwilla, Dorothee J., Colin M. Brown, and Peter Hagoort. 1995. The N400 as a function of the level of processing. In Psychophysiology 32 (3): 274–285.

Coles, Michael G.H., and Michael D. Rugg. 1996. “Event-related brain potentials: An introduction”. In Electrophysiology of mind, ed. Michael D. Rugg and Michael G. H. Coles, 1–26. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

DeLong, Katherine A., Thomas P. Urbach, and Marta Kutas. 2005. Probabilistic word pre-activation during language comprehension inferred from electrical brain activity. In Nature neuroscience 8 (8): 1117–1121.

Drange, Theodore. 1966. Type crossings: Sentential meaninglessness in the border area of linguistics and philosophy. The Hague: Mouton.

Book  Google Scholar 

Dudschig, Carolin, Claudia Maienborn, and Barbara Kaup. 2016. Is there a difference between stripy journeys and stripy ladybirds? The N400 response to semantic and world-knowledge violations during sentence processing. In Brain and cognition 103: 38–49.

Elbourne, Paul. 2016. Multi-sentential category mistakes. In Inquiry 59 (5): 542–558.

Federmeier, Kara D., and Marta Kutas. 1999. A rose by any other name: Long-term memory structure and sentence processing. In Journal of memory and language 41 (4): 469–495.

Fodor, J.A., and J.J. Katz. 1963. The structure of a semantic theory. In Language 39 (2): 170–210.

Friederici, A.D. 1995. The time course of syntactic activation during language processing: A model based on neuropsychological and neurophysiological data. In Brain and language 50 (3): 259–281.

Goddard, L., and R. Routley. 1973. The logic of significance and context. Edinburgh: Scottish Academic Press.

Google Scholar 

Goldwater, Jonah. 2018. Ryle, the double counting problem, and the logical form of category mistakes. In Journal of the history of philosophy 56 (2): 337–359.

Hagoort, Peter, Lea Hald, Marcel Bastiaansen, and Karl Magnus Petersson. 2004. Integration of word meaning and world knowledge in language comprehension. In Science 304 (5669): 438–441.

Hagoort, Peter. 2003. Interplay between syntax and semantics during sentence comprehension: ERP effects of combining syntactic and semantic violations. In Journal of cognitive neuroscience 15 (6): 883–899.

Hillyard, Steven A., and J.C. Hansen. 1986. Attention: Electrophysiological approaches. In Psychophysiology: Systems, processes, and applications, ed. M.G.H. Coles, E. Donchin, and S.W. Porges, 227–243. New York, NY: Guilford.

Google Scholar 

Hodapp, Alice, and Milena Rabovsky. 2021. The N400 ERP component reflects an error-based implicit learning signal during language comprehension. In European journal of neuroscience 54 (9): 7125–7140.

Hoeks, John C.J.., Laurie A. Stowe, and Gina Doedens. 2004. Seeing words in context: The Interaction of lexical and sentence level information during reading. In Cognitive brain research 19 (1): 59–73.

Holcomb, Philip J., and Warren B. McPherson. 1994. Event-related brain potentials reflect semantic priming in an object decision task. In Brain and cognition 24 (2): 259–276.

Kaan, Edith, Anthony Harris, Edward Gibson, and Phillip Holcomb. 2000. The P600 as an index of syntactic integration difficulty. In Language and cognitive processes 15 (2): 159–201.

Kim, Albert, and Lee Osterhout. 2005. The Independence of combinatory semantic processing: Evidence from Event-Related Potentials. In Journal of memory and language 52 (2): 205–225.

Kornhuber, Hans H., and Lüder Deecke. 1965. “Hirnpotentialänderungen BeiWillkürbewegungen Und Passiven Bewegungen Des Menschen: Bereitschaftspotential Und Reafferente Potentiale”. In Pflüger’ Archiv für die gesamte Physiologie des Menschen und der Tiere, 284.1, pp. 1–17.

Kuperberg, Gina R., Tatiana Sitnikova, David Caplan, and Phillip J. Holcomb. 2003. Electrophysiological distinctions in processing conceptual relationships within simple sentences. In Cognitive brain research 17 (1): 117–129.

Kuperberg, Gina R., David Caplan, Tatiana Sitnikova, Marianna Eddy, and Phillip J. Holcomb. 2006. Neural correlates of processing syntactic, semantic, and thematic relationships in sentences. In Language and cognitive processes 21 (5): 489–530.

Kuperberg, Gina R., Donna A. Kreher, Tatiana Sitnikova, David N. Caplan, and Phillip J. Holcomb. 2007. The role of animacy and thematic relationships in processing active English sentences: Evidence from event-related potentials. In Brain and language 100 (3): 223–237.

Kutas, Marta, and Kara D. Federmeier. 2000. Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension. In Trends in cognitive sciences 4 (12): 463–470.

Kutas, Marta, and Steven A. Hillyard. 1980. Reading between the lines: Event-related brain potentials during natural sentence processing. In Brain and language 11 (2): 354–373.

Kutas, Marta, and Steven A. Hillyard. 1980. Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. In Science 207 (4427): 203–205.

Kutas, Marta, and Steven A. Hillyard. 1984. Brain potentials during reading reflect word expectancy and semantic association. In Nature 307 (5947): 161–163.

Kutas, Marta, Cyma K. van Petten, and M. Besson. 1988. Event-related potential asymmetries during the reading of sentences. In Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology 69 (3): 218–233.

Kutas, Marta, Cyma K. van Petten, and Robert Kluender. 2006. Psycholinguistics electrified II (1994–2005). In Handbook of psycholinguistics, ed. Matthew J. Traxler and Morton A. Gernsbacher, 659–724. London: Academic Press.

Chapter  Google Scholar 

Lappin, Shalom. 1981. Sorts, ontology, and metaphor: The semantics of sortal structure. Berlin & New York, NY: De Gruyter

Loveless, Norman E., and Anthony J. Sanford. 1974. Effects of age on the contingent negative variation and preparatory set in a reaction-time task. In Journal of gerontology 29 (1): 52–63.

Magidor, Ofra. 2016. Response to Abrusán, Shaw, and Elbourne. In Inquiry 59 (5): 559–586.

Magidor, Ofra. 2013. Category mistakes. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Book  Google Scholar 

Martin, John. 1975. A Many-valued semantics for category mistakes. In Synthese 31 (1): 63–83.

Martin, Robert L. 1974. Sortal ranges for complex predicates. In Journal of Philosophical Logic 3 (1): 159–167.

Näätänen, Risto, Anthony W. K. Gaillard, and Sirkka Mäntysalo. 1978. Early selective-attention effect on evoked potential reinterpreted. In Acta psychologica 42 (4): 313–329.

Neville, Helen, Janet L. Nicol, Andrew Barss, Kenneth I. Forster, and Merrill F. Garrett. 1991. Syntactically based sentence processing classes: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. In Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 3 (2): 151–165.

Nieuwland, Mante S., and Jos J. A. van Berkum. 2006. When peanuts fall in love: N400 evidence for the power of discourse. In Journal of cognitive neuroscience 18 (7): 1098–1111.

Nieuwland, Mante S., Stephen Politzer-Ahles, Evelien Heyselaar, Katrien Segaert, Emily Darley, Nina Kazanina, Sarah Von Grebmer Zu Wolfsthurn, Frederica Bartolozzi, Vita Kogan, Aine Ito, Diane Mézière, Dale J. Barr, Guillaume A. Rousselet, Heather J. Ferguson, Simon Busch-Moreno, Xiao Fu, Jyrki Tuomainen, Eugenia Kulakova, E Matthew Husband, David I. Donaldson, Zdenko Kohút, Shirley-Ann Rueschemeyer, and Falk Huettig. 2018. Large-scale replication study reveals a limit on probabilistic prediction in language comprehension. In eLife 7: 1–24.

Nieuwland, Mante S., Dale J. Barr, Frederica Bartolozzi, Simon Busch-Moreno, Emily Darley, David I. Donaldson, Heather J. Ferguson, Xiao Fu, Evelien Heyselaar, Falk Huettig, E Matthew Husband, Aine Ito, Nina Kazanina, Vita Kogan, Zdenko Kohút, Eugenia Kulakova, Diane Mézière, Stephen Politzer-Ahles, Guillaume A. Rousselet, Shirley-Ann Rueschemeyer, Katrien Segaert, Jyrki Tuomainen, and Sarah Von Grebmer Zu Wolfsthurn. 2020. Dissociable effects of prediction and integration during language comprehension: Evidence from a large-scale study using brain potentials. In Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 375 (1791): 1–9.

Nieuwland, Mante S., Yana Arkhipova, and Pablo Rodríguez-Gómez. 2020. Anticipating words during spoken discourse comprehension: A large-scale, pre-registered replication study using brain potentials. In Cortex 133: 1–36.

Nigam, Arti, James E. Hoffman, and Robert F. Simons. 1992. N400 to semantically anomalous pictures and words. In Journal of cognitive neuroscience 4 (1): 15–22.

Osterhout, Lee, and Janet Nicol. 1999. On the distinctiveness, independence, and time course of the brain responses to syntactic and semantic anomalies. In Language and cognitive processes 14 (3): 283–317.

Osterhout, Lee, and Phillip J. Holcomb. 1992. Event-related brain potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly. In Journal of memory and language 31 (6): 785–806.

Osterhout, Lee, Phillip J. Holcomb, and David A. Swinney. 1994. Brain potentials elicited by garden-path sentences: Evidence of the application of verb information during parsing. In Journal of experimental psychology: Learning, memory, and cognition 20 (4): 786–803.

Pylkkänen, Liina, and Alec Marantz. 2003. Tracking the time course of word recognition with MEG. In Trends in cognitive sciences 7 (5): 187–189.

Rabovsky, Milena, Steven S. Hansen, and James L. McClelland. 2018. Modelling the N400 brain potential as change in a probabilistic representation of meaning. In Nature human behaviour 2 (9): 693–705.

Rugg, Michael D. and Michael G. H. Coles. 1996. The ERP and cognitive psychology: Conceptual issues. In Electrophysiology of mind. ed. Michael D. Rugg and Michael G. H. Coles, 27–39 Oxford: Oxford University Press

Ryle, Gilbert. 1938. Categories. In Proceedings of the Aristotelian society 38: 189–206.

Ryle, Gilbert. 1949. The Concept of Mind. London; New York, NY: Hutchinson

Shaw, James R. 2016. Magidor on anomaly and truth-value gaps. In Inquiry 59 (5): 513–528.

St. George, Marie and Suzanne Mannes. 1994. Global semantic expectancy and language comprehension. In Journal of cognitive neuroscience 6 (1): 70–83.

Stroud, C .M. A. 2009. Structural and semantic selectivity in the electrophysiology of sentence comprehension. PhD thesis. University of Maryland.

Sutton, Samuel, Margery Braren, Joseph Zubin, and E. R. John. 1965. Evoked-potential correlates of stimulus uncertainty. In Science 150 (3700): 1187–1188.

Szabó, Zoltán Gendler. 2015. Review of Magidor’s ‘Category Mistakes’. In Philosophical review 124 (2): 289–292.

Thomason, Richmond H. 1972. A semantic theory of sortal incorrectness. In Journal of philosophical logic 1 (2): 209–258.

van Berkum, Jos J. A. 2009. The ’neuropragmatics’ of simple utterance comprehension: An ERP Review. In Semantics and pragmatics: From experiment to theory, ed. U. Sauerland and K. Yatsushiro, 276–316. New York, NY: Palgrave Macmillan.

van Berkum, Jos J. A., Peter Hagoort, and Colin M. Brown. 1999. Semantic integration in sentences and discourse: Evidence from the N400. In Journal of cognitive neuroscience 11 (6): 657–671.

van Berkum, Jos J. A. 2004. Sentence comprehension in a wider discourse: Can we use ERPs to keep track of things? In The On-line Study of Sentence Comprehension: Eyetracking, ERPs and Beyond. ed. Manuel Carreiras and Charles Clifton Jr, 229–270 New York, NY: Psychology Press

van Berkum, Jos J. A., Pienie Zwitserlood, Peter Hagoort, and Colin M. Brown. 2003. When and how do listeners relate a sentence to the wider discourse? Evidence from the N400 Effect. In Cognitive brain research 17 (3): 701–718.

van Fraassen, Bas C. 1971. Formal Semantics and Logic. New York, NY: Macmillan.

van Petten, Cyma K., and Marta Kutas. 1990. Interactions between sentence context and word frequency in event-related brain potentials. In Memory & Cognition 18 (4): 380–393.

Walter, W. G., R. Cooper, V. J. Aldridge, W. C. McCallum, and A. L. Winter. 1964. Contingent negative variation: An electric sign of sensori-motor association and expectancy in the human brain. In Nature 230: 380–384.

Willems, Roel M., and Aslı Özyürek, and Peter Hagoort. 2008. Seeing and hearing meaning: ERP and fMRI evidence of word versus picture integration into a sentence context. In Journal of cognitive neuroscience 20 (7): 1235–1249.

留言 (0)

沒有登入
gif