Background/Aims: Many motor impairments are present in older adults with cognitive decline. One of them is the impairment of hand dexterity and bimanual coordination that result in poor functional ability in the activities of daily living (ADL). This study investigated the effects of hand dexterity and bimanual coordination declination on the sub-domains of ADL in older adults with mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Methods: Thirty-one senior individuals with MCI were recruited in this study. The Purdue Pegboard Test was used to measure hand dexterity, and bimanual coordination was assessed by the continuous circle-drawing task. Their ADL were assessed with the General Activity Daily Living questionnaire. Results: The correlations analysis showed an association between the dominant hand and bimanual dexterity with the domestic domain of ADL and all conditions of hand dexterity with the complex domain of ADL. Moreover, the multiple regression analysis showed that the predictor of the greatest effect for domestic and complex domains was dominant hand dexterity. Discussion/Conclusion: These results revealed that dominant hand dexterity strongly affected domestic and complex ADL in older adults with MCI. There were age-related changes regarding lateral asymmetrical motor reduction, especially in cognitive tasks. However, complex tasks involving cognitive function may need dominant, nondominant and bimanual hand dexterity.
© 2022 The Author(s). Published by S. Karger AG, Basel
IntroductionCognitive impairment is a major clinical and public health problem that involves the impairment of episodic memory, languages, and problem-solving skills. More importantly, it affects the quality of life in older adults and their families [1]. Thus, it is important to identify the factors that may be affected after cognitive impairment.
Patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) exhibit a progressive impairment of episodic memory and other cognitive capability, which affects their functional abilities [2]. However, in the early stages of AD, also known as mild cognitive impairment (MCI), they show a slightly impaired cognitive capability and functional abilities [3]. Many motor impairments may emerge as a phenotype of cognitive decline [4]. A mild decline in fine hand motor function and complex hand motor function were found in older adults and affected to less able to control or adjust the amount force to fit the task, which is essential for activities of daily living (ADL) such as pouring milk, removing money from a wallet, and writing. Especially, hand dexterity is an important component to completely perform the tasks in ADL and the hand dexterity showed a decrease in older adults with MCI [5-9]. Changes in brain from neurodegenerative dementia (e.g., brain atrophy, neuronal loss, or synaptic dysfunction) lead to difficulty to learn movement causing both cognitive and motor dysfunction in MCI [10]. A deficit in the dominant hand, nondominant hand, or bimanual dexterity is associated with functional loss, particularly affecting the deficit of instrumental ADL (IADL) [11]. IADL require more complex neurophysiological processing capacity than basic ADL (BADL) and therefore, are more prone to deterioration triggered by cognitive decline and seem to be most severely affected in elderly with MCI more than without MCI. The elderly with MCI requires more time and less accuracy while solving tasks such as planning a bus route, packing specific item in a picnic basket, or taking medication [12]. The nature of hand movement requires visual search [5], motor speed skill [6], as well as attention allocation and motor planning [13]. Therefore, the main important functional skills, which are to control the attention, working memory, planning, judgment, task flexibility, and inhibition [14], seem to be strongly influenced by the performance of hand functions as a result of the reflection on the skilled fingers in grasping, lifting, and manipulating objects. Many daily bimanual tasks require precise force adjustments for stabilization and manipulation as seen in the hand dexterity, grip strength, or bimanual coordination.
Moreover, a bimanual task presents a challenge for fine motor control and requires the precise spatiotemporal coordination of both hands. Age-related declinations in coordination depend on the type of tasks and bimanual performance [15-17]. Older adults exhibit a greater breakdown in temporal coordination and slower movement in execution time than younger adults, especially in conditions requiring bimanual asymmetrical coordination [16]. In general, bimanual functional skills depend on the intra and interhemispheric neural networks involving the primary motor cortex, supplementary motor area, premotor cortex, cingulate, and posterior parietal cortex [18]. These areas are necessary for controlling bimanual movement and the connection between two hemispheres via the corpus collosum [19]. Structural and functional changes in the aging brain include the corpus collosum that affects bimanual coordination. From magnetic resonance imaging studies, the appearance of widespread gray and white matter deteriorations have been shown in aging [20] and a pronounced progression in the frontal area [21]. The older adults who have exhibited cognitive decline demonstrate an even larger reduction of connectivity between the intra and interhemispheric neural networks [22-24]. A continuous bimanual circle-drawing that relies on the coordination between hand functions has been investigated in previous studies [25, 26]. They have found that continuous circle-drawing was not different between young and older adults. However, there are few studies reported on the effects of changes in the intra and interhemispheric connections controlling bimanual coordination [26-28].
Current data on the brain control function in each and in bimanual hands for performing ADL in older adults with MCI is limited. Therefore, the aim of this study was to investigate whether age-related hand functional impairments as a result of the reduction of intra and interhemispheric neural connections have any impacts on the hand performance to perform sub-domain of ADL impairment. This study determined the effects of hand dexterity and bimanual coordination declination on the sub-domain of ADL in older adults with MCI.
Materials and MethodsParticipantsThis study was carried out at a selected community in Nakhon Si Thammarat from March to August 2020. A total of 49 participants were initially recruited in this study, 18 participants were excluded from this study which consists of a participant showed sensation impairment, a participant showed anxiety and depression, and 16 participants had Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) score more than 26 or less than 17 (cutoff score was set at 26 had sensitivity of 90% to detect MCI [29]), as showed in Figure 1. Total of 31 participants (11 males and 20 females) with different socioeconomic backgrounds gave consent to participate in the study. The inclusion criteria were (1) participants were in the age-group of 60–79 years old; (2) participants had right-handed dominance; and (3) participants were well oriented to time, place, and persons. All participants gave written informed consent for their participation and for the assessment of their cognitive level by the MoCA, and the assessment of their anxiety and depression status via face-to-face interview. All information was clearly explained to the participants along with questions and answers with detailed information of the protocol prior to the study. The study protocol was proved by the Ethics Committee of Walailak University (Reference No. WUEC-20-069-01).
Fig. 1.Hand dexterity was assessed using the Perdue Pegboard Test (PPT: Lafayette Instrument Co., Model 30,020) and the standardized and reliable assessment for hand dexterity [30]. Briefly, it consists of a 19.7 × 44.9 cm board with 25 slotted holes in a 5 × 5 array. The participants were instructed to insert one pin at a time starting from the top hole in either the right or left row, depending on the starting hand except in two-hands condition where the participants have to pick up one pin in the right bottom hole and insert it into the right row with the right hand whereby the left hand also picks up one pin in the left bottom hole and inserts it into the left row with the left hand. Participants have to complete each exercise within 30 s with no option to pick up any dropped pins. The number of pins was recorded after filling in the holes and the highest number of pins of either the right, left, or bimanual hands condition was recorded.
Bimanual Coordination AssessmentBimanual coordination was used to assess the continuous circle-drawing task as reported from a previous study [26]. In brief, two target circles were displayed side by side on a wooden board with a distance of 22 cm. The inner and outer diameter of each circle was 6 and 8 cm, respectively. A white fixation cross was mid-positioned between the two circles. A red vertical line at the top of each circle was the starting point of the assessment. Participants were instructed to draw a circle with a pen held by each hand and drawn in a counterclockwise direction. Then, they were also instructed to focus their eyes on the white fixation cross to minimize head movement and try to keep the hand within their lines between the inner and outer diameter of the circle and to be careful to avoid touching the edge of the circle. The drawing time was recorded for one round from the starting point.
General Activity Daily Living AssessmentThe standard questionnaire for General Activity Daily Living (GADL) was used to evaluate functional ADL for each participant by face-to-face interview. In short, the GADL shows a hierarchical structure with three components of more specific activities as demonstrated in the previous study [31]. In summary, the first component is BADL or self-care ADL, including changing clothes, using the toilet, bathing or showering, getting into/out of bed or a chair, and eating. The GADL covers the IADL; the first component of IADL is the domestic domain including the ability of doing household chores, telephone calling, meals preparation, and doing the laundry. The second component of IADL is the complex domain including the ability to manage financial matters, to shop, to take medication, and to go out alone on public transportation. Each domain showed good internal consistency (>0.800) and domestic and complex’s accuracy was higher for the distinction between MCI and AD. This assessment included an interview with the participants to investigate their ability to respond in each activity. The participants were also instructed to provide their accurate answers for each activity.
The scoring for the evaluation of each activity was divided into three scales: 2 means the participant is independent to perform the activities without help or supervision, 1 means the participant partially needs supervision, help, or special equipment to perform the activities, and 0 means the participant needs constant helps, supervision, or special equipment to perform the activities.
ProcedureThe participants were screened for their cognitive level by the MoCA, including their anxiety and depression status. Since educational adjustment has been found to affect the MoCA scores [32], the level of cognitive education was categorized as 1–4, 5–8, 9–12, and more than 12 years. However, the education effects of 1 point were added for participants with 12 years of education on their MoCA score (if <30).
All 31 eligible participants sat comfortably on their chair and placed their arms on a table in a position ready to reach and pick up an object in front of them. The procedure was first explained and the assessment of PPT was demonstrated step by step until the participants clearly understood the overall activity. Within 30 s, the participants needed to complete each condition consisting of the dominant hand, nondominant hand, and bimanual dexterity. The order of the three conditions of the PPT was randomized by the researchers before testing. Upon completion of PPT, these participants were further explained and demonstrated the assessment of the continuous circle-drawing test. The participants were instructed to draw a circle with two hands simultaneously for a total of three times. Their time spent was recorded in each activity and the average time was then calculated. Afterward, the participants were interviewed face-to-face and instructed to truthfully answer the questions for each item of the GADL questionnaire. The summarized protocol was shown in online supplementary material (see www.karger.com/doi/10.1159/000521644 for all online suppl. material).
Data AnalysisAll data were recorded and entered using the statistical software version 20.0 of SPSS to determine the correlations between hand dexterity and bimanual coordination with each domain of GADL. Descriptive data-included demographic and clinical characteristics were analyzed using percentage and mean ± SD. A multiple linear regression analysis with a forced stepwise model was used to test whether the performance of hand functions on each hand, bimanual dexterity, and bimanual coordination could predict the score from each domain of GADL including the score of dominant hand dexterity, nondominant hand dexterity, bimanual dexterity, and the elapsed time of bimanual coordination. The association between hand dexterity and bimanual coordination was analyzed using Pearson correlations to evaluate if the contribution of hand dexterity and bimanual coordination to ADL performance was independent. A value of p < 0.05 was regarded as statistically significant.
ResultsWe evaluated 31 participants who exhibited MCI. The majority of the subjects are of low socioeconomic status, of an age range of 60–69 years old, possess less than 4 years of formal education, and have underlying diseases. The baseline demographic characteristics are shown in Table 1.
Table 1.Baseline demographic characteristic of the study subjects
Table 2 shows the sociodemographic and clinical data, ADL domain scores, the average score for hand dexterity, and the elapsed time for bimanual coordination. Thirty-one participants who exhibited MCI had an average MoCA score of 20.55 ± 2.11. The average age was 68.39 ± 4.50 years; none of them presented depression and anxiety symptoms. The mean TGDS-15 was 2.35 ± 1.70.
Table 2.Mean values of selected parameters
The PPT scores for the three conditions (dominant hand, nondominant hand, and bimanual dexterity) were 12.13 ± 1.59, 11.45 ± 2.14, and 6.90 ± 1.72, respectively. The GADL scores for the domestic and complex domains were 7.52 ± 0.811 and 7.45 ± 0.675, respectively. The average time of bimanual coordination was 6.40 ± 2.02 s. However, the average score for the self-care domain of the GADL assessment was 10.00 ± 0.00, which represented no functional decline in older adults with MCI. Analysis of older adults with MCI should not include the self-care or BADL domain; therefore, we did not include this domain in the data analysis and discussion.
Correlations between Domestic Domain with Hand Dexterity and CoordinationThere was a significant correlation between the domestic domain with dominant hand dexterity (p = 0.001, r = 0.516) and bimanual dexterity (p = 0.020, r = 0.372) as showed in Figure 2. However, there was no correlation found between nondominant hand dexterity and bimanual coordination with the domestic domain.
Fig. 2.Association between domestic domain of GADL and dominant hand and bimanual dexterity. The correlations of the domestic domain of GADL showed a significance correlation with the dominant hand (a) and bimanual hands dexterity (b) (r = 0.516, 0.372, respectively). The number of dots in the scatterplot differs from the sample size due to superposed values.
In the regression analysis, the model was significant for the domestic domain of GADL (F = 10.55, p = 0.003, R2 = 0.267) and the predictor of the greatest effect for the domestic domain was dominant hand dexterity (p = 0.003). Nondominant hand dexterity, bimanual dexterity, and bimanual coordination were not predictors for the domestic domain (p > 0.05) as showed in Table 3.
Table 3.Linear regression models of hand dexterity and bimanual coordination as predicted effects of hand performance declination on ADL
There was a significant correlation between the complex domain with dominant hand dexterity (p < 0.001, r = 0.566), nondominant hand dexterity (p = 0.002, r = 0.500), and bimanual dexterity (p = 0.025, r = 0.355) as shown in Figure 3. However, we did not find any correlation between bimanual coordination with the complex domain.
Fig. 3.Association between complex domain of GADL and dominant hand and bimanual dexterity. The correlations of the complex domain of GADL showed a significance correlation with the dominant hand (a), nondominant hand (b), and bimanual hands dexterity (c) (r = 0.566 (a), 0.500 (b), and 0.355 (c), respectively). The number of dots in the scatterplot differs from the sample size due to superposed values.
In the regression analysis, the model was significant for the complex domain of GADL (F = 13.69, p = 0.001, R2 = 0.321) and the predictor of greatest effect for the complex domain was dominant hand dexterity (p = 0.001). Nondominant hand dexterity, bimanual dexterity, and bimanual coordination were not predictors for the complex domain (p > 0.05) as showed in Table 3.
DiscussionThe correlation between specific motor declines and poor ADL shows limited reports particularly on older adults with MCI. Our results found correlations between hand dexterity with domestic and complex ADL. Moreover, we found that only dominant hand dexterity had a significant influence on the performance of domestic and complex ADL.
Interestingly, our finding shows a similarity on decreasing the hand dexterity with a previous study that PPT in healthy older adults revealed an asymmetrical reduction of hand dexterity in each and both sides of the right, left and both hands, respectively [30]. Furthermore, the average PPT of the right, left, and both hands was 14.3, 13.7, and 10.9 and 12.7, 12.7, and 10.2 in the age-group of 60–70 years for females and males, respectively [26]. PPT in the dominant hand in healthy older adults demonstrated the average values of 14.5, 13.7, 13.1, and 11.9 in the age-groups of 60–64, 65–69, 70–74, and ≥75 years, respectively [33].
The asymmetrical declination between hemispheres has been explained in earlier studies. Kalisch et al. [34] designed an experiment to investigate age-related changes in hand dominance in individuals aged 20–90 years. They measured motor performance and recorded the use of the hand in everyday activities. They found that right-hand dominance declined with aging and the participants showed more balance between using the right hand and left hand at an advanced age than younger ones [34, 35]. A greater decline in hand performance was found in the dominant hand at an advancing age and with older adults who could no longer maintain the intensity and load that they once favored with the dominant hand.
Moreover, similar results also reported the asymmetrical brain declination from a study done by Przybyla et al. [36]. Their findings showed a decreased tendency of the dominant hand and balance with using the right and left hand at an advanced age. Age-related changes in regard to lateral asymmetrical motor reduction occurred in older adults, and dominant hand performance remained the same for hand path curvature while nondominant hand performance decreased in older adults compared to younger ones. Moreover, the accuracy and precision of the dominant hand decreased from young adults and showed balance in advanced age [36]. The general finding is that prefrontal cortex activity during cognitive tasks is less lateralized in older adults compared to younger adults, which potentially contributes to the HAROLD model. The greater bilateral recruitment that occurs in older adults was associated with better task performance, whereas those who performed poorly on cognitive tasks exhibited an asymmetrical pattern in their neural activity [36]. It is plausible that the reduced motor asymmetry occurs in older adults. Mattay et al. [37] showed increased ipsilateral activation patterns in older adults when compare to young adults during a simple unilateral pressing task.
The present study found there was no association between bimanual coordination with domestic and complex ADL. This finding is supported by age-related changes in interlimb coordination, as indicated by bilateral motor synergies during isometric force control for two visual conditions (yes and no visual feedback) [38]. It is noteworthy that the older adults revealed higher values of force variability in asymmetry and correlation of interlimb coordination in the older adults than the younger adults [38]. Nondominant hand force variation, controlled by right hemisphere, is responsible for modulating the stability of limb movement. During the bilateral movement, interhemispheric connection by the corpus collosum function balanced interhemispheric inhibition for successful task performance [28]. In addition, a smaller volume of the corpus collosum and reduced white matter integrity was found in older adults [39]. Thus, these structural and functional changes (less interhemispheric connectivity) in the corpus callosum may lead to more functional motor impairments in interlimb coordination [27]. Another possibility was the cerebellum, which controls bilateral motor synergy and enhances motor coordination and online-motor correction based on error learning [40]. Presumably, aging influences cerebellar functions that may directly (or indirectly) interfere with bilateral motor synergies.
Our results suggested that the motor impairment, which concerns hand dexterity or bimanual coordination, was a consequence of cognitive decline in older adults with MCI. Complex ADL concerns tasks that need cognitive function and more stability and precision skills, so it correlates with the nondominant hand to maintain the skills. In particular, the dominant hand dexterity was the most effective predictor for predicting the ability to perform ADL, whether in the domestic or complex domain. However, bimanual coordination did not observe a correlation with ADL. Previous studies have reported that a smaller volume of the corpus collosum may lead to impairment in interlimb coordination in older adults [39].
Our study had limitations that should be addressed. First, the small sample size may not generally provide much significant findings to support the overall study. A large scale of sample size is definitely required in future studies before any conclusion could be made. Second, this was a cross-sectional study that may not be able to complete all information, especially a predictive model on motor function decline. In future studies, we could assess motor declination in long-term motor changes that are affected by ADL or set a comparison group including young adults or healthy older adults to investigate the tendency for changes in motor function. Lastly, we included independent parameters which may not be sufficient to represent all activities in ADL. Therefore, in future studies, we could consider more parameters such as gait speed, balance or eye-hand coordination to verify those associations before any conclusion could be made.
ConclusionBased on the results obtained from this study, older adults with MCI showed their abilities on self-care or BADL. Hand dexterity was found in correlation with domestic and complex ADL. Moreover, the dominant hand dexterity strongly affects the domestic and complex domains of ADL. This preliminary study therefore provides important information to offer; practical benefits in using cognitive and motor assessment would allow the clinician to compute better estimate of functional performance and help to predict ADL deterioration in elderly with MCI. Elderly with MCI who impaired motor dexterity may be at higher risk of loss of IADL performance, and physical training program focusing fine motor dexterity may improve performance in these ADLs.
AcknowledgmentsThe authors would like to thank to all the participants who sacrificed their time and excellently participated in this study. Moreover, we would especially like to grateful to Miss Piyaphat Chinaphandu for her assistance in data collection. Finally, we would like to give our utmost sincere thanks to the head of senior association in Thasala Sub-distinct, Nakhon Si Thammarat, Thailand.
Statement of EthicsThis study was conducted in conformity with the World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Ethics Committee of Walailak University (Reference No. WUEC-20-069-01). All participants have agreed with written informed consent.
Conflict of Interest StatementThere is no conflict of interest to declare.
Funding SourcesThis study was supported by Walailak University research Grant (WU-IRG-63-057). The funder had no role in the study design, data collection, discussion, or preparation of the manuscript.
Author ContributionsBecause this study has an author, Prathomchai Rattanawan designed the study, collected data, planned and performed the statistical analyses, interpreted the results, and wrote the manuscript. The author received suggestions about editing English’s grammar from his advisor.
Data Availability StatementAll data generated or analyzed during this study are included in this Google form link: https://docs.google.com/spreadsheets/d/1mlUh1nIhZyOaHWj7Q4Er6YvzUwDfxnCZfS6RArcfJSQ/edit?usp=sharing The link was set to only see.
This article is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License (CC BY-NC). Usage and distribution for commercial purposes requires written permission. Drug Dosage: The authors and the publisher have exerted every effort to ensure that drug selection and dosage set forth in this text are in accord with current recommendations and practice at the time of publication. However, in view of ongoing research, changes in government regulations, and the constant flow of information relating to drug therapy and drug reactions, the reader is urged to check the package insert for each drug for any changes in indications and dosage and for added warnings and precautions. This is particularly important when the recommended agent is a new and/or infrequently employed drug. Disclaimer: The statements, opinions and data contained in this publication are solely those of the individual authors and contributors and not of the publishers and the editor(s). The appearance of advertisements or/and product references in the publication is not a warranty, endorsement, or approval of the products or services advertised or of their effectiveness, quality or safety. The publisher and the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to persons or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content or advertisements.
Comments (0)