Allopenna, P. D., Magnuson, J. S., Tanenhaus, M. K. (1998). Tracking the time course of spoken word recognition using eye movements: Evidence for continuous mapping models. Journal of Memory and Language, 38(4), 419–439.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
ISI
Andrews, S. (1992). Frequency and neighborhood effects on lexical access: Lexical similarity or orthographic redundancy? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(2), 234–254.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
ISI
Apfelbaum, K. S., Klein-Packard, J., McMurray, B. (2021). The pictures who shall not be named: Empirical support for benefits of preview in the Visual World Paradigm. Journal of Memory and Language, 121, Article 104279.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Apfelbaum, K. S., McMurray, B. (2017). Learning during processing: Word learning doesn’t wait for word recognition to finish. Cognitive Science, 41(S4), 706–747.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Balota, D. A., Cortese, M. J., Sergent-Marshall, S. D., Spieler, D. H., Yap, M. J. (2004). Visual word recognition of single-syllable words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(2), 283–316.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline |
ISI
Brysbaert, M., New, B. (2009). Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 977–990.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline |
ISI
Carreiras, M., Perea, M. (2002). Masked priming effects with syllabic neighbors in a lexical decision task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28(5), 1228–1242.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline |
ISI
Carreiras, M., Perea, M., Grainger, J. (1997). Effects of the orthographic neighborhood in visual word recognition: Cross-task comparisons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23(4), 857–871.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline |
ISI
Chambers, S. M. (1979). Letter and order information in lexical access. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18(2), 225–241.
Google Scholar |
Crossref
Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108(1), 204–256.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline |
ISI
Cooper, R . (1974). The control of eye fixation by the meaning of spoken language. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 84–107.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
ISI
Dahan, D., Magnuson, J. S. (2006). Spoken-word recognition. In Taxler, M. J., Gernsbacher, A. (Eds.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 249–283). Academic Press.
Google Scholar |
Crossref
Dahan, D., Magnuson, J. S., Tanenhaus, M. K. (2001). Time course of frequency effects in spoken-word recognition: Evidence from eye movements. Cognitive Psychology, 42(4), 317–367.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Dare, N., Shillcock, R. (2013). Serial and parallel processing in reading: Investigating the effects of parafoveal orthographic information on nonisolated word recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(3), 487–504.
Google Scholar |
SAGE Journals |
ISI
Davis, C. J. (1999). The self-organizing lexical acquisition and recognition (SOLAR) model of visual word recognition [Doctoral dissertation, University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW, Australia].
Google Scholar
Davis, C. J. (2010). The spatial coding model of visual word identification. Psychological Review, 117(3), 713–758.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Davis, C. J., Lupker, S. J. (2006). Masked inhibitory priming in English: Evidence for lexical inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(3), 668–687.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
De Moor, W., Brysbaert, M. (2000). Neighborhood-frequency effects when primes and targets are of different lengths. Psychological Research, 63(2), 159–162.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Drews, E., Zwitserlood, P. (1995). Morphological and orthographic similarity in visual word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(5), 1098–1116.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Dufour, S., Grainger, J. (2019). Phoneme-order encoding during spoken word recognition: A priming investigation. Cognitive Science, 43(10), Article e12785.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Evett, L. J., Humphreys, G. W. (1981). The use of abstract graphemic information in lexical access. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33(4), 325–350.
Google Scholar |
SAGE Journals
Farris-Trimble, A., McMurray, B. (2013). Test–retest reliability of eye tracking in the visual world paradigm for the study of real-time spoken word recognition. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56(4), 1328–1345.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Forster, K. I., Davis, C. (1991). The density constraint on form-priming in the naming task: Interference effects from a masked prime. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 1–25.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
ISI
Forster, K. I., Davis, C., Schoknecht, C., Carter, R. (1987). Masked priming with graphemically related forms: Repetition or partial activation? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39(2), 211–251.
Google Scholar |
SAGE Journals
Forster, K. I., Shen, D. (1996). No enemies in the neighborhood: Absence of inhibitory neighborhood effects in lexical decision and semantic categorization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(3), 696–713.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline |
ISI
Forster, K. I., Veres, C. (1998). The prime lexicality effect: Form-priming as a function of prime awareness, lexical status, and discrimination difficulty. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24(2), 498–514.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Frauenfelder, U. H., Scholten, M., Content, A. (2001). Bottom-up inhibition in lexical selection: Phonological mismatch effects in spoken word recognition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16(5–6), 583–607.
Google Scholar |
Crossref
Gaskell, M. G., Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1997). Integrating form and meaning: A distributed model of speech perception. Language and Cognitive Processes, 12(5–6), 613–656.
Google Scholar |
Crossref
Goh, W. D., Yap, M. J., Chee, Q. W. (2020). The Auditory English Lexicon Project: A multi- talker, multi-region psycholinguistic database of 10,170 spoken words and nonwords. Behavior Research Methods, 52, 2202–2231.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Grainger, J. (1992). Orthographic neighborhoods and visual word recognition. In. Advances in Psychology, 94, 131–146.
Google Scholar |
Crossref
Grainger, J . (2008). Cracking the orthographic code: An introduction. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23(1), 1–35.
Google Scholar |
Crossref
Grainger, J., Dufau, S., Ziegler, J. C. (2016). A vision of reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(3), 171–179.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Grainger, J., Colé, P., Segui, J. (1991). Masked morphological priming in visual word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 30(3), 370–384.
Google Scholar |
Crossref
Grainger, J., Ferrand, L. (1994). Phonology and orthography in visual word recognition: Effects of masked homophone primes. Journal of Memory and Language, 33(2), 218–233.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
ISI
Grainger, J., Ferrand, L. (1996). Masked orthographic and phonological priming in visual word recognition and naming: Cross-task comparisons. Journal of Memory and Language, 35(5), 623–647.
Google Scholar |
Crossref
Grainger, J., Jacobs, A. M. (1996). Orthographic processing in visual word recognition: A multiple read-out model. Psychological Review, 103(3), 518–586.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline |
ISI
Grainger, J., Mathôt, S., Vitu, F. (2014). Tests of a model of multi-word reading: Effects of parafoveal flanking letters on foveal word recognition. Acta Psychologica, 146, 35–40.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline |
ISI
Grainger, J., Van Heuven, W. (2003). Modeling letter position coding in printed word perception. In Bonin, P. (Ed.), The mental lexicon (pp. 2–23). Nova Science.
Google Scholar
Grainger, J., Whitney, C. (2004). Does the huamn mnid raed wrods as a wlohe? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(2), 58–59.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Gregg, J., Inhoff, A. W., Connine, C. M. (2019). Re-reconsidering the role of temporal order in spoken word recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72(11), 2574–2583.
Google Scholar |
SAGE Journals |
ISI
Hannagan, T., Magnuson, J. S., Grainger, J. (2013). Spoken word recognition without a TRACE. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, Article 563.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Harm, M. W., Seidenberg, M. S. (1999). Phonology, reading acquisition, and dyslexia: Insights from connectionist models. Psychological Review, 106(3), 491–528.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Hendrickson, K., Spinelli, J., Walker, E. (2020). Cognitive processes underlying spoken word recognition during soft speech. Cognition, 198, Article 104196.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Humphreys, G. W., Evett, L. J., Quinlan, P. T., Besnar, D. (1987). Orthographic priming qualitative differences between priming from identified and unidentified primes. In Colheart, M. (Ed.), Attention and performance XII: The psychology of reading (pp. 201–219). Lawrence Erlbaum.
Google Scholar
Huntsman, L. A., Lima, S. D. (1996). Orthographic neighborhood structure and lexical access. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 25(3), 417–429.
Google Scholar |
Crossref
Johnson, R. L., Eisler, M. E. (2012). The importance of the first and last letter in words during sentence reading. Acta Psychologica, 141(3), 336–351.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Jordan, T. R., Thomas, S. M., Patching, G. R., Scott-Brown, K. C. (2003). Assessing the importance of letter pairs in initial, exterior, and interior positions in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(5), 883–893.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Luce, P. A., Goldinger, S. D., Auer, E. T., Vitevitch, M. S. (2000). Phonetic priming, neighborhood activation, and PARSYN. Perception & Psychophysics, 62(3), 615–625.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline
Luce, P. A., Pisoni, D. B. (1998). Recognizing spoken words: The neighborhood activation model. Ear and Hearing, 19(1), 1–36.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline |
ISI
Luck, S. J., Vogel, E. K. (1997). The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions. Nature, 390(6657), 279–281.
Google Scholar |
Crossref |
Medline |
ISI
Magnuson, J. S. (2019). Fixations in the visual world paradigm: Where, when, why? Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science, 3, 1–27.
Google Scholar |
Crossref
Marian, V., Bartolotti, J., Chabal, S., Shook, A. (2012). CLEARPOND: Cross-linguistic easy-access resource for phonological and orthographic neighborhood densities. PLOS ONE, 7(8), Article e43230.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0043230 Google Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W., Zwitserlood, P. (1989). Accessing spoken words: The importance of word onsets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15(3), 576–585.
Comments (0)